Priming plural ambiguities. Maldonado, M., Chemla, E., & Spector, B. Journal of Memory and Language, 95:89 - 101, 2017.
Paper
Journal link
More doi abstract bibtex 12 downloads Sentences that involve plural expressions, such as numerical expressions, give rise to systematic ambiguities. For example, the sentence Two boys have three balloons can either mean that there are two boys who, between them, have three balloons (cumulative reading) or that there are two boys who each have three balloons (distributive reading). In this paper, we report the results of three experiments which show that the distributive/cumulative ambiguity can give rise to priming effects. That is, when subjects perform a sentence-picture matching task which creates a strong bias towards one of the two types of readings, they are more likely to access the very same type of interpretation when subsequently presented with a different sentence-picture pair which does not create a similar bias. This finding suggests that the abstract constructs that linguists posit to account for different types of readings describe some real features of mental representations.
@article{Maldonado:2017aa,
abstract = {Sentences that involve plural expressions, such as numerical expressions, give rise to systematic ambiguities. For example, the sentence Two boys have three balloons can either mean that there are two boys who, between them, have three balloons (cumulative reading) or that there are two boys who each have three balloons (distributive reading). In this paper, we report the results of three experiments which show that the distributive/cumulative ambiguity can give rise to priming effects. That is, when subjects perform a sentence-picture matching task which creates a strong bias towards one of the two types of readings, they are more likely to access the very same type of interpretation when subsequently presented with a different sentence-picture pair which does not create a similar bias. This finding suggests that the abstract constructs that linguists posit to account for different types of readings describe some real features of mental representations.},
author = {Mora Maldonado and Emmanuel Chemla and Benjamin Spector},
date-added = {2017-02-21 23:55:23 +0000},
doi = {10.1016/j.jml.2017.02.002},
issn = {0749-596X},
journal = {Journal of Memory and Language},
keywords = {Semantics},
pages = {89 - 101},
title = {Priming plural ambiguities},
url = {http://semanticsarchive.net/Archive/ThhYTBiO/Maldonado-Chemla-Spector-Priming-Plural-Ambiguities.pdf},
url_journal_link = {http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0749596X17300098},
url_more = {http://semanticsarchive.net/Archive/ThhYTBiO/primingplurals.html},
volume = {95},
year = {2017},
bdsk-url-1 = {http://semanticsarchive.net/Archive/ThhYTBiO/paper.pdf}}
Downloads: 12
{"_id":"9yx4L4tu8jq6Fehsb","bibbaseid":"maldonado-chemla-spector-primingpluralambiguities-2017","downloads":12,"creationDate":"2017-03-07T08:44:09.721Z","title":"Priming plural ambiguities","author_short":["Maldonado, M.","Chemla, E.","Spector, B."],"year":2017,"bibtype":"article","biburl":"http://www.emmanuel.chemla.free.fr/Material/EmmanuelChemlaBibliography.bib","bibdata":{"bibtype":"article","type":"article","abstract":"Sentences that involve plural expressions, such as numerical expressions, give rise to systematic ambiguities. For example, the sentence Two boys have three balloons can either mean that there are two boys who, between them, have three balloons (cumulative reading) or that there are two boys who each have three balloons (distributive reading). In this paper, we report the results of three experiments which show that the distributive/cumulative ambiguity can give rise to priming effects. That is, when subjects perform a sentence-picture matching task which creates a strong bias towards one of the two types of readings, they are more likely to access the very same type of interpretation when subsequently presented with a different sentence-picture pair which does not create a similar bias. This finding suggests that the abstract constructs that linguists posit to account for different types of readings describe some real features of mental representations.","author":[{"firstnames":["Mora"],"propositions":[],"lastnames":["Maldonado"],"suffixes":[]},{"firstnames":["Emmanuel"],"propositions":[],"lastnames":["Chemla"],"suffixes":[]},{"firstnames":["Benjamin"],"propositions":[],"lastnames":["Spector"],"suffixes":[]}],"date-added":"2017-02-21 23:55:23 +0000","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2017.02.002","issn":"0749-596X","journal":"Journal of Memory and Language","keywords":"Semantics","pages":"89 - 101","title":"Priming plural ambiguities","url":"http://semanticsarchive.net/Archive/ThhYTBiO/Maldonado-Chemla-Spector-Priming-Plural-Ambiguities.pdf","url_journal_link":"http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0749596X17300098","url_more":"http://semanticsarchive.net/Archive/ThhYTBiO/primingplurals.html","volume":"95","year":"2017","bdsk-url-1":"http://semanticsarchive.net/Archive/ThhYTBiO/paper.pdf","bibtex":"@article{Maldonado:2017aa,\n\tabstract = {Sentences that involve plural expressions, such as numerical expressions, give rise to systematic ambiguities. For example, the sentence Two boys have three balloons can either mean that there are two boys who, between them, have three balloons (cumulative reading) or that there are two boys who each have three balloons (distributive reading). In this paper, we report the results of three experiments which show that the distributive/cumulative ambiguity can give rise to priming effects. That is, when subjects perform a sentence-picture matching task which creates a strong bias towards one of the two types of readings, they are more likely to access the very same type of interpretation when subsequently presented with a different sentence-picture pair which does not create a similar bias. This finding suggests that the abstract constructs that linguists posit to account for different types of readings describe some real features of mental representations.},\n\tauthor = {Mora Maldonado and Emmanuel Chemla and Benjamin Spector},\n\tdate-added = {2017-02-21 23:55:23 +0000},\n\tdoi = {10.1016/j.jml.2017.02.002},\n\tissn = {0749-596X},\n\tjournal = {Journal of Memory and Language},\n\tkeywords = {Semantics},\n\tpages = {89 - 101},\n\ttitle = {Priming plural ambiguities},\n\turl = {http://semanticsarchive.net/Archive/ThhYTBiO/Maldonado-Chemla-Spector-Priming-Plural-Ambiguities.pdf},\n\turl_journal_link = {http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0749596X17300098},\n\turl_more = {http://semanticsarchive.net/Archive/ThhYTBiO/primingplurals.html},\n\tvolume = {95},\n\tyear = {2017},\n\tbdsk-url-1 = {http://semanticsarchive.net/Archive/ThhYTBiO/paper.pdf}}\n\n","author_short":["Maldonado, M.","Chemla, E.","Spector, B."],"key":"Maldonado:2017aa","id":"Maldonado:2017aa","bibbaseid":"maldonado-chemla-spector-primingpluralambiguities-2017","role":"author","urls":{"Paper":"http://semanticsarchive.net/Archive/ThhYTBiO/Maldonado-Chemla-Spector-Priming-Plural-Ambiguities.pdf"," journal link":"http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0749596X17300098"," more":"http://semanticsarchive.net/Archive/ThhYTBiO/primingplurals.html"},"keyword":["Semantics"],"metadata":{"authorlinks":{"maldonado, m":"https://moramaldonado.github.io/","chemla, e":"http://www.emmanuel.chemla.free.fr/"}},"downloads":12},"search_terms":["priming","plural","ambiguities","maldonado","chemla","spector"],"keywords":["semantics"],"authorIDs":["xjLcwkTWA9wrD3tjQ","zWrRBQxp6ofKNusXt"],"dataSources":["bnajr6hRNBqcyhXEP","tjiGpM3QkXrAR3DJb","Wqf2wymPCfn8MEnTR"]}