@article{fant_stable_2022, title = {Stable cooperation emerges in stochastic multiplicative growth}, url = {http://arxiv.org/abs/2202.02787}, abstract = {Understanding the evolutionary stability of cooperation is a central problem in biology, sociology, and economics. There exist only a few known mechanisms that guarantee the existence of cooperation and its robustness to cheating. Here, we introduce a new mechanism for the emergence of cooperation in the presence of fluctuations. We consider agents whose wealth change stochastically in a multiplicative fashion. Each agent can share part of her wealth as public good, which is equally distributed among all the agents. We show that, when agents operate with long time-horizons, cooperation produce an advantage at the individual level, as it effectively screens agents from the deleterious effect of environmental fluctuations.}, language = {en}, urldate = {2022-02-08}, journal = {arXiv:2202.02787 [cond-mat, physics:physics, q-bio, q-fin]}, author = {Fant, Lorenzo and Mazzarisi, Onofrio and Panizon, Emanuele and Grilli, Jacopo}, month = feb, year = {2022}, note = {arXiv: 2202.02787}, keywords = {unread}, }
@article{bairagya_eco-evolutionary_2022, title = {Eco-evolutionary games for harvesting self-renewing common resource: {Effect} of growing harvester population}, shorttitle = {Eco-evolutionary games for harvesting self-renewing common resource}, url = {http://arxiv.org/abs/2205.04877}, abstract = {The tragedy of the commons (TOC) is a ubiquitous social dilemma witnessed in interactions between a population of living entities and shared resources available to them: The individuals in the population tend to selfishly overexploit a common resource as it is arguably the rational choice, or in case of non-human beings, it may be an evolutionarily uninvadable action. How to avert the TOC is a significant problem related to the conservation of resources. It is not hard to envisage situations where the resource could be self-renewing and the size of the population may be dependent on the state of the resource through the fractions of the population employing different exploitation rates. If the self-renewal rate of the resource lies between the maximum and the minimum exploitation rates, it is not a priori obvious under what conditions the TOC can be averted. In this paper, we address this question analytically and numerically using the setup of an evolutionary game theoretical replicator equation that models the Darwinian tenet of natural selection. Through the replicator equation, while we investigate how a population of replicators exploit the shared resource, the latter's dynamical feedback on the former is also not ignored. We also present a transparent bottom-up derivation of the game-resource feedback model to facilitate future studies on the stochastic effects on the findings presented herein.}, urldate = {2022-05-11}, journal = {arXiv:2205.04877 [nlin, physics:physics, q-bio]}, author = {Bairagya, Joy Das and Mondal, Samrat Sohel and Chowdhury, Debashish and Chakraborty, Sagar}, month = may, year = {2022}, note = {arXiv: 2205.04877}, keywords = {unread}, }
@article{menezes_how_2022, title = {How local antipredator response unbalances the rock-paper-scissors model}, url = {http://arxiv.org/abs/2202.00149}, abstract = {Antipredator behaviour is a self-preservation strategy present in many biological systems, where individuals join the effort in a collective reaction to avoid being caught by an approaching predator. We study a nonhierarchical tritrophic system, whose predator-prey interactions are described by the rock-paper-scissors game rules. We performed a set of spatial stochastic simulations where organisms of one out of the species can resist predation in a collective strategy. The drop in predation capacity is local, which means that each predator faces a particular opposition depending on the prey group size surrounding it. Considering that the interference in a predator action depends on the prey's physical and cognitive ability, we explore the role of a conditioning factor that indicates the fraction of the species apt to perform the antipredator strategy. Because of the local unbalancing of the cyclic predator-prey interactions, departed spatial domains mainly occupied by a single species emerge. Unlike the rock-paper-scissors model with a weak species because a nonlocal reason, our findings show that if the predation probability of one species is reduced because individuals face local antipredator response, the species does not predominate. Instead, the local unbalancing of the rock-paper-scissors model results in the prevalence of the weak species' prey. Finally, the outcomes show that local unevenness may jeopardise biodiversity, with the coexistence being more threatened for high mobility.}, urldate = {2022-02-03}, journal = {arXiv:2202.00149 [nlin, physics:physics, q-bio]}, author = {Menezes, J. and Batista, S. and Tenorio, M. and Triaca, E. A. and Moura, B.}, month = jan, year = {2022}, note = {arXiv: 2202.00149}, keywords = {Nonlinear Sciences - Pattern Formation and Solitons, Quantitative Biology - Populations and Evolution, Quantitative Biology - Quantitative Methods, unread}, }
@article{di_carlo_evidence_2022, title = {Evidence of fluctuation-induced first-order phase transition in active matter}, url = {http://arxiv.org/abs/2202.01010}, abstract = {We study the critical properties of the Malthusian Toner-Tu theory in its near ordering phase. Because of the birth/death process, characteristic of this Malthusian model, density fluctuations are partially suppressed, and the theory can be studied with perturbative renormalization group techniques. We compute the renormalization group flow equations for the Malthusian Toner-Tu theory at one-loop order. The renormalization group flow drives the system in an unstable region, suggesting a fluctuation-induced first-order phase transition. This calculation could provide a rigorous RG explanation of why the disorder/order transition is first order in active matter systems.}, urldate = {2022-02-03}, journal = {arXiv:2202.01010 [cond-mat]}, author = {Di Carlo, Luca and Scandolo, Mattia}, month = feb, year = {2022}, note = {arXiv: 2202.01010}, keywords = {unread}, }
@article{menezes_pattern_2022, title = {Pattern formation and coarsening dynamics in apparent competition models}, url = {http://arxiv.org/abs/2202.05293}, abstract = {Apparent competition is an indirect interaction between species that share natural resources without any mutual aggression but negatively affect each other if there is a common enemy. The negative results of the apparent competition are reflected in the species spatial segregation, which impacts the dynamics of their populations. Performing a series of stochastic simulations, we study a model where organisms of two prey species do not compete for space but share a common predator. Our outcomes elucidate the central role played by the predator in the pattern formation and coarsening dynamics in apparent competition models. Investigating the effects of predator mortality on the persistence of the species, we find a crossover between a curvature driven scaling regime and a coexistence scenario. For low predator mortality, spatial domains mainly inhabited by one type of prey arise, surrounded by interfaces that mostly contain predators. We demonstrate that the dynamics of the interface network are curvature driven whose coarsening follows a scaling law common to other nonlinear systems. The effects of the apparent competition decrease for high predator mortality, allowing organisms of two prey species to share a more significant fraction of lattice. Finally, our results reveal that predation capacity in single-prey domains influences the scaling power law that characterises the coarsening dynamics. Our findings may be helpful to biologists to understand the pattern formation and dynamics of biodiversity in systems with apparent competition.}, language = {en}, urldate = {2022-02-15}, journal = {arXiv:2202.05293 [nlin, physics:physics, q-bio]}, author = {Menezes, J. and Moura, B.}, month = feb, year = {2022}, note = {arXiv: 2202.05293}, keywords = {unread}, }
@misc{zamponi_scale_2022, title = {Scale free density and correlations fluctuations in the dynamics of large microbial ecosystems}, url = {http://arxiv.org/abs/2206.12384}, abstract = {Microorganisms self-organize in very large communities exhibiting complex fluctuations. Despite recent advances, still the mechanism by which these systems are able to exhibit large variability at the one hand and dynamical robustness on the other, is not fully explained. With that motivation, here we analyze three aspects of the dynamics of the microbiota and plankton: the density fluctuations, the correlation structure and the avalanching dynamics. In all communities under study we find that the results exhibits scale-free density fluctuations, anomalous variance' scaling, scale-free abundance correlations and stationary scale-free avalanching dynamics. These behaviors, typical in systems exhibiting critical dynamics, suggest criticality as a potential mechanism to explain both the robustness and (paradoxical) high irregularity of processes observed in very large microbial communities.}, urldate = {2022-06-27}, publisher = {arXiv}, author = {Zamponi, Nahuel and Grigera, Tomas S. and Gudowska-Nowak, Ewa and Chialvo, Dante R.}, month = jun, year = {2022}, note = {arXiv:2206.12384 [q-bio]}, keywords = {unread}, }
@article{burkart_periodic_2022, title = {Periodic temporal environmental variations induce coexistence in resource competition models}, url = {http://arxiv.org/abs/2202.11635}, abstract = {Natural ecosystems, in particular on the microbial scale, are inhabited by a large number of species. The population size of each species is affected by interactions of individuals with each other and by spatial and temporal changes in environmental conditions, such as resource abundance. Here, we use a generic population dynamics model to study how, and under what conditions, a periodic temporal environmental variation can alter an ecosystem's composition and biodiversity. We demonstrate that using time scale separation allows one to qualitatively predict the long-term population dynamics of interacting species in varying environments. We show that the notion of competitive exclusion, a well-known principle that applies for constant environments, can be extended to temporally varying environments if the time scale of environmental changes (e.g., the circadian cycle of a host) is much faster than the time scale of population growth (doubling time in bacteria). When these time scales are similar, our analysis shows that a varying environment deters the system from reaching a steady state, and coexistence between multiple species becomes possible. Our results posit that biodiversity can in parts be attributed to natural environmental variations.}, urldate = {2022-02-24}, journal = {arXiv:2202.11635 [q-bio]}, author = {Burkart, Tom and Frey, Erwin}, month = feb, year = {2022}, note = {arXiv: 2202.11635}, keywords = {on website, unread}, }
@misc{amirian_extending_2022, title = {Extending the {Monod} {Model} of {Microbial} {Growth} with {Memory}}, url = {http://arxiv.org/abs/2207.02028}, abstract = {Monod's model describes the growth of microorganisms using a hyperbolic function of extracellular resource concentration. Under fluctuating or limited resource concentrations this model performs poorly against experimental data, motivating the more complex Droop model with a time-varying internal storage pool. We extend the Monod model to incorporate memory of past conditions, adding a single parameter motivated by a fractional calculus analysis. We show how to interpret the memory element in a biological context and describe its connection to a resource storage pool. Under nitrogen starvation at non-equilibrium conditions, we validate the model with simulations and empirical data obtained from lab cultures of diatoms (T. pseudonana and T. weissflogii) and prasinophytes (Micromonas sp. and O. tauri), globally influential phytoplankton taxa. Using statistical analysis, we show that our Monod-memory model estimates the growth rate, cell density, and resource concentration as well as the Droop model while requiring one less state variable. Our simple model may improve descriptions of phytoplankton dynamics in complex earth system models at a lower computational cost than is presently achievable.}, urldate = {2022-07-06}, publisher = {arXiv}, author = {Amirian, Mohammad M. and Irwin, Andrew J. and Finkel, Zoe V.}, month = jul, year = {2022}, note = {arXiv:2207.02028 [math, q-bio, stat]}, keywords = {unread}, }
@article{baumgarten_toy_2022, title = {A toy model for brain criticality: self-organized excitation/inhibition ratio and the role of network clustering}, shorttitle = {A toy model for brain criticality}, url = {http://arxiv.org/abs/2202.03330}, abstract = {The critical brain hypothesis receives increasing support from recent experimental results. It postulates that the brain is at a critical point between an ordered and a chaotic regime, sometimes referred to as the "edge of chaos." Another central observation of neuroscience is the principle of excitation-inhibition balance: Certain brain networks exhibit a remarkably constant ratio between excitation and inhibition. When this balance is perturbed, the network shifts away from the critical point, as may for example happen during epileptic seizures. However, it is as of yet unclear what mechanisms balance the neural dynamics towards this excitation-inhibition ratio that ensures critical brain dynamics. Here we introduce a simple yet biologically plausible toy model of a self-organized critical neural network with a self-organizing excitation to inhibition ratio. The model only requires a neuron to have local information of its own recent activity and changes connections between neurons accordingly. We find that the network evolves to a state characterized by avalanche distributions following universal scaling laws typical of criticality, and to a specific excitation to inhibition ratio. The model connects the two questions of brain criticality and of a specific excitation/inhibition balance observed in the brain to a common origin or mechanism. From the perspective of the statistical mechanics of such networks, the model uses the excitation/inhibition ratio as control parameter of a phase transition, which enables criticality at arbitrary high connectivities. We find that network clustering plays a crucial role for this phase transition to occur.}, language = {en}, urldate = {2022-02-08}, journal = {arXiv:2202.03330 [cond-mat, physics:nlin, q-bio]}, author = {Baumgarten, Lorenz and Bornholdt, Stefan}, month = feb, year = {2022}, note = {arXiv: 2202.03330}, keywords = {unread}, }
@article{nemati_moran_2022, title = {Moran dynamics in spatially heterogeneous environments with periodic fitness distribution}, url = {http://arxiv.org/abs/2202.04501}, abstract = {Local environmental interactions are a major factor in determining the success of a new mutant in structured populations. Spatial variations of concentration of a resource change the fitness of competing strategies locally and thus can drastically change the outcome of evolutionary process in unintuitive ways. Environmental interactions can be asymmetric, i.e. the same local resource value affects the fitness of strategies differently. The question is how such local environmental variations in network population structures change the condition for selection and fixation probability of an advantageous (or deleterious) mutant. We consider linear graph structure and focus on the case where resources have a spatial periodic pattern. Our model covers several biologically relevant cases. We numerically calculate fixation probability and fixation time for a Moran birth-death process as fitness heterogeneity and period vary. The fixation probability is affected by not only the level of fitness heterogeneity, but also spatial scale of resource variations set by period of distribution \$T\$. For most (weak) asymmetric environmental interactions the chance of success of a mutant increases with heterogeneity. We identify conditions for which a previously deleterious mutant (in a uniform environment) becomes beneficial as fitness heterogeneity is increased. We observe cases where the fixation probability of both mutant and resident types are less than their neutral value, \$1/N\$, simultaneously. This corresponds to potential coexistence of resident and mutant types. Finally, we discuss the effect of 'fitness shift' where the fitness function of two types has a phase difference. This happens when there are more than one type of resources in the environment. We observe significant increase (or decrease) in the fixation probability of the mutant as a result of such phase shift.}, language = {en}, urldate = {2022-02-10}, journal = {arXiv:2202.04501 [physics, q-bio]}, author = {Nemati, Hossein and Ejtehadi, Mohammad Reza and Kaveh, Kamran}, month = feb, year = {2022}, note = {arXiv: 2202.04501}, keywords = {unread, on website}, file = {Nemati et al. - 2022 - Moran dynamics in spatially heterogeneous environm.pdf:C\:\\Users\\matth\\Zotero\\storage\\2ZPU4GWJ\\Nemati et al. - 2022 - Moran dynamics in spatially heterogeneous environm.pdf:application/pdf}, }
@article{rangel_combination_2022, title = {Combination of survival movement strategies in cyclic game systems during an epidemic}, url = {http://arxiv.org/abs/2203.00133}, abstract = {Disease outbreaks affect many ecosystems threatening species that also fight against other natural enemies. We investigate a cyclic game system with \$5\$ species, whose organisms outcompete according to the rules of a generalised spatial rock-paper-scissors game, during an epidemic. We study the effects of behavioural movement strategies that allow individuals of one out of the species to move towards areas with a low density of disease vectors and a high concentration of enemies of their enemies. We perform a series of stochastic simulations to discover the impact of self-preservation strategies in pattern formation, calculating the species' spatial autocorrelation functions. Considering organisms with different physical and cognitive abilities, we compute the benefits of each movement tactic to reduce selection and infection risks. Our findings show that the maximum profit in terms of territorial dominance in the cyclic game is achieved if both survival movement strategies are combined, with individuals prioritising social distancing. In the case of an epidemic causing symptomatic illness, the drop in infection risk when organisms identify and avoid disease vectors does not render a rise in the species population because many refuges are disregarded, limiting the benefits of safeguarding against natural enemies. Our results may be helpful to the understanding of the behavioural strategies in ecosystems where organisms adapt to face living conditions changes.}, urldate = {2022-03-03}, journal = {arXiv:2203.00133 [nlin, physics:physics, q-bio]}, author = {Rangel, E. and Moura, B. and Menezes, J.}, month = feb, year = {2022}, note = {arXiv: 2203.00133}, keywords = {unread}, }
@article{murray_global_2022, title = {Global burden of bacterial antimicrobial resistance in 2019: a systematic analysis}, volume = {399}, issn = {0140-6736, 1474-547X}, shorttitle = {Global burden of bacterial antimicrobial resistance in 2019}, url = {https://www.thelancet.com/journals/lancet/article/PIIS0140-6736(21)02724-0/fulltext}, doi = {10.1016/S0140-6736(21)02724-0}, language = {English}, number = {10325}, urldate = {2022-03-15}, journal = {The Lancet}, author = {Murray, Christopher JL and Ikuta, Kevin Shunji and Sharara, Fablina and Swetschinski, Lucien and Aguilar, Gisela Robles and Gray, Authia and Han, Chieh and Bisignano, Catherine and Rao, Puja and Wool, Eve and Johnson, Sarah C. and Browne, Annie J. and Chipeta, Michael Give and Fell, Frederick and Hackett, Sean and Haines-Woodhouse, Georgina and Hamadani, Bahar H. Kashef and Kumaran, Emmanuelle A. P. and McManigal, Barney and Agarwal, Ramesh and Akech, Samuel and Albertson, Samuel and Amuasi, John and Andrews, Jason and Aravkin, Aleskandr and Ashley, Elizabeth and Bailey, Freddie and Baker, Stephen and Basnyat, Buddha and Bekker, Adrie and Bender, Rose and Bethou, Adhisivam and Bielicki, Julia and Boonkasidecha, Suppawat and Bukosia, James and Carvalheiro, Cristina and Castañeda-Orjuela, Carlos and Chansamouth, Vilada and Chaurasia, Suman and Chiurchiù, Sara and Chowdhury, Fazle and Cook, Aislinn J. and Cooper, Ben and Cressey, Tim R. and Criollo-Mora, Elia and Cunningham, Matthew and Darboe, Saffiatou and Day, Nicholas P. J. and Luca, Maia De and Dokova, Klara and Dramowski, Angela and Dunachie, Susanna J. and Eckmanns, Tim and Eibach, Daniel and Emami, Amir and Feasey, Nicholas and Fisher-Pearson, Natasha and Forrest, Karen and Garrett, Denise and Gastmeier, Petra and Giref, Ababi Zergaw and Greer, Rachel Claire and Gupta, Vikas and Haller, Sebastian and Haselbeck, Andrea and Hay, Simon I. and Holm, Marianne and Hopkins, Susan and Iregbu, Kenneth C. and Jacobs, Jan and Jarovsky, Daniel and Javanmardi, Fatemeh and Khorana, Meera and Kissoon, Niranjan and Kobeissi, Elsa and Kostyanev, Tomislav and Krapp, Fiorella and Krumkamp, Ralf and Kumar, Ajay and Kyu, Hmwe Hmwe and Lim, Cherry and Limmathurotsakul, Direk and Loftus, Michael James and Lunn, Miles and Ma, Jianing and Mturi, Neema and Munera-Huertas, Tatiana and Musicha, Patrick and Mussi-Pinhata, Marisa Marcia and Nakamura, Tomoka and Nanavati, Ruchi and Nangia, Sushma and Newton, Paul and Ngoun, Chanpheaktra and Novotney, Amanda and Nwakanma, Davis and Obiero, Christina W. and Olivas-Martinez, Antonio and Olliaro, Piero and Ooko, Ednah and Ortiz-Brizuela, Edgar and Peleg, Anton Yariv and Perrone, Carlo and Plakkal, Nishad and Ponce-de-Leon, Alfredo and Raad, Mathieu and Ramdin, Tanusha and Riddell, Amy and Roberts, Tamalee and Robotham, Julie Victoria and Roca, Anna and Rudd, Kristina E. and Russell, Neal and Schnall, Jesse and Scott, John Anthony Gerard and Shivamallappa, Madhusudhan and Sifuentes-Osornio, Jose and Steenkeste, Nicolas and Stewardson, Andrew James and Stoeva, Temenuga and Tasak, Nidanuch and Thaiprakong, Areerat and Thwaites, Guy and Turner, Claudia and Turner, Paul and Doorn, H. Rogier van and Velaphi, Sithembiso and Vongpradith, Avina and Vu, Huong and Walsh, Timothy and Waner, Seymour and Wangrangsimakul, Tri and Wozniak, Teresa and Zheng, Peng and Sartorius, Benn and Lopez, Alan D. and Stergachis, Andy and Moore, Catrin and Dolecek, Christiane and Naghavi, Mohsen}, month = feb, year = {2022}, pmid = {35065702}, note = {Publisher: Elsevier}, keywords = {unread, ⛔ No INSPIRE recid found}, pages = {629--655}, file = {Full Text PDF:C\:\\Users\\matth\\Zotero\\storage\\LCFE27SP\\Murray et al. - 2022 - Global burden of bacterial antimicrobial resistanc.pdf:application/pdf;Snapshot:C\:\\Users\\matth\\Zotero\\storage\\W7A63ABC\\fulltext.html:text/html}, }
@article{menezes_aggregation_2022, title = {Aggregation as an antipredator strategy in the rock-paper-scissors model}, url = {http://arxiv.org/abs/2202.12371}, abstract = {We study a nonhierarchical tritrophic system, whose predator-prey interactions are described by the rock-paper-scissors game rules. In our stochastic simulations, individuals may move strategically towards the direction with more conspecifics to form clumps instead of moving aimlessly on the lattice. Considering that the conditioning to move gregariously depends on the organism's physical and cognitive abilities, we introduce a maximum distance an individual can perceive the environment and a minimum conditioning level to perform the gregarious movement. We investigate the pattern formation and compute the average size of the single-species spatial domains emerging from the grouping behaviour. The results reveal that the defence tactic reduces the predation risk significantly, being more profitable if individuals perceive further distances, thus creating bigger groups. Our outcomes show that the species with more conditioned organisms dominate the cyclic spatial game, controlling most of the territory. On the other hand, the species with fewer individuals ready to perform aggregation strategy gives its predator the chance to fill the more significant fraction of the grid. The spatial interactions assumed in our numerical experiments constitute a data set that may help biologists and data scientists understand how local interactions influence ecosystem dynamics.}, urldate = {2022-02-28}, journal = {arXiv:2202.12371 [nlin, physics:physics, q-bio]}, author = {Menezes, J. and Rangel, E. and Moura, B.}, month = feb, year = {2022}, note = {arXiv: 2202.12371}, keywords = {unread}, }
@article{zheng_probabilistic_2022, title = {Probabilistic fair behaviors spark its boost in the {Ultimatum} {Game}: the strength of good {Samaritans}}, shorttitle = {Probabilistic fair behaviors spark its boost in the {Ultimatum} {Game}}, url = {http://arxiv.org/abs/2202.06002}, abstract = {Behavioral experiments on the Ultimatum Game have shown that we human beings have remarkable preference in fair play, contradicting the predictions by the game theory. Most of the existing models seeking for explanations, however, strictly follow the assumption of {\textbackslash}emph\{Homo economicus\} in orthodox Economics that people are self-interested and fully rational to maximize their earnings. Here we relax this assumption by allowing that people probabilistically choose to be "good Samaritans", acting as fair players from time to time. For well-mixed and homogeneously structured populations, we numerically show that as this probability increases the level of fairness undergoes from the low scenario abruptly to the full fairness state, where occasional fair behaviors (\${\textbackslash}sim5{\textbackslash}\%\$) are sufficient to drive the whole population to behave in the half-half split manner. We also develop a mean-field theory, which correctly reproduces the first-order phase transition and points out that the bistability is an intrinsic property of this game and small fair acts lead to dramatical change due to its bifurcation structure. Heterogeneously structured populations, however, display continuous fairness transition; surprisingly, very few hub nodes acting as fair players are able to entrain the whole population to the full fairness state. Our results thus reveal the unexpected strength of "good Samaritans", which may constitute a new explanation for the emergence of fairness in our society.}, language = {en}, urldate = {2022-02-15}, journal = {arXiv:2202.06002 [cond-mat, q-bio]}, author = {Zheng, Guozhong and Zhang, Jiqiang and Liang, Rizhou and Ma, Lin and Chen, Li}, month = feb, year = {2022}, note = {arXiv: 2202.06002}, keywords = {unread}, }
@article{evans_exactly_2022, title = {An exactly solvable predator prey model with resetting}, url = {http://arxiv.org/abs/2202.06138}, abstract = {We study a simple model of a diffusing particle (the prey) that on encounter with one of a swarm of diffusing predators can either perish or be reset to its original position at the origin. We show that the survival probability of the prey up to time \$t\$ decays algebraically as \${\textbackslash}sim t{\textasciicircum}\{-{\textbackslash}theta(p, {\textbackslash}gamma)\}\$ where the exponent \${\textbackslash}theta\$ depends continuously on two parameters of the model, with \$p\$ denoting the probability that a prey survives upon encounter with a predator and \${\textbackslash}gamma = D\_A/(D\_A+D\_B)\$ where \$D\_A\$ and \$D\_B\$ are the diffusion constants of the prey and the predator respectively. We also compute exactly the probability distribution \$P(N{\textbar}t\_c)\$ of the total number of encounters till the capture time \$t\_c\$ and show that it exhibits an anomalous large deviation form \$P(N{\textbar}t\_c){\textbackslash}sim t\_c{\textasciicircum}\{- {\textbackslash}Phi{\textbackslash}left({\textbackslash}frac\{N\}\{{\textbackslash}ln t\_c\}=z{\textbackslash}right)\}\$ for large \$t\_c\$. The rate function \${\textbackslash}Phi(z)\$ is computed explicitly. Numerical simulations are in excellent agreement with our analytical results.}, language = {en}, urldate = {2022-02-15}, journal = {arXiv:2202.06138 [cond-mat]}, author = {Evans, Martin R. and Majumdar, Satya N. and Schehr, Grégory}, month = feb, year = {2022}, note = {arXiv: 2202.06138}, keywords = {unread}, }
@article{adachi_universal_2022, title = {Universal constraint on nonlinear population dynamics}, url = {http://arxiv.org/abs/2202.02028}, abstract = {Ecological and evolutionary processes show various population dynamics depending on internal interactions and environmental changes. While crucial in predicting biological processes, discovering general relations for such nonlinear dynamics has remained a challenge. Here, we derive a universal information-theoretical constraint on a broad class of nonlinear dynamical systems represented as population dynamics. The constraint is interpreted as a generalization of Fisher's fundamental theorem of natural selection. Furthermore, the constraint indicates nontrivial bounds for the speed of critical relaxation around bifurcation points, which we argue are universally determined only by the type of bifurcation. Our theory is verified for an evolutionary model and an epidemiological model, which exhibit the transcritical bifurcation, as well as for an ecological model, which undergoes limit-cycle oscillation. This work paves a way to predict biological dynamics in light of information theory, by providing fundamental relations in nonequilibrium statistical mechanics of nonlinear systems.}, language = {en}, urldate = {2022-02-07}, journal = {arXiv:2202.02028 [cond-mat, physics:physics]}, author = {Adachi, Kyosuke and Iritani, Ryosuke and Hamazaki, Ryusuke}, month = feb, year = {2022}, note = {arXiv: 2202.02028}, keywords = {unread}, }
@article{mavrodiev_enhanced_2021, title = {Enhanced or distorted wisdom of crowds? {An} agent-based model of opinion formation under social influence}, volume = {15}, issn = {1935-3812, 1935-3820}, shorttitle = {Enhanced or distorted wisdom of crowds?}, url = {https://link.springer.com/10.1007/s11721-021-00189-3}, doi = {10.1007/s11721-021-00189-3}, abstract = {We propose an agent-based model of collective opinion formation to study the wisdom of crowds under social influence. The opinion of an agent is a continuous positive value, denoting its subjective answer to a factual question. The wisdom of crowds states that the average of all opinions is close to the truth, i.e., the correct answer. But if agents have the chance to adjust their opinion in response to the opinions of others, this effect can be destroyed. Our model investigates this scenario by evaluating two competing effects: (1) agents tend to keep their own opinion (individual conviction), (2) they tend to adjust their opinion if they have information about the opinions of others (social influence). For the latter, two different regimes (full information vs. aggregated information) are compared. Our simulations show that social influence only in rare cases enhances the wisdom of crowds. Most often, we find that agents converge to a collective opinion that is even farther away from the true answer. Therefore, under social influence the wisdom of crowds can be systematically wrong.}, language = {en}, number = {1-2}, urldate = {2022-02-03}, journal = {Swarm Intelligence}, author = {Mavrodiev, Pavlin and Schweitzer, Frank}, month = jun, year = {2021}, keywords = {unread}, pages = {31--46}, }
@article{westbrook_dopamine_2020, title = {Dopamine promotes cognitive effort by biasing the benefits versus costs of cognitive work}, volume = {367}, issn = {0036-8075, 1095-9203}, url = {https://www.science.org/doi/10.1126/science.aaz5891}, doi = {10.1126/science.aaz5891}, abstract = {Responsible use of psychostimulants Psychostimulants have a place in the therapy of attentional disorders. However, they are also widely used off-label to enhance cognitive performance, and their mechanisms of action remain elusive. Westbrook et al. studied the effects of these drugs and concurrently measured striatal dopamine synthesis capacity in young, healthy participants (see the Perspective by Janes). They administered a placebo, methylphenidate (a dopamine and noradrenaline reuptake blocker), and sulpiride (a selective D2 receptor antagonist) while participants made explicit cost-benefit decisions about whether to engage in cognitive effort. Higher dopamine synthesis capacity in the caudate nucleus was associated with greater willingness to allocate cognitive effort. In addition, methylphenidate and sulpiride increased subjective values and motivation to work specifically for people with low dopamine synthesis capacity. Cognition-enhancing drugs may thus act at the motivational level rather than directly boosting cognition per se. Science , this issue p. 1362 ; see also p. 1300 , Striatal dopamine increases cognitive effort by amplifying and attenuating the subjective benefits and costs, respectively, of cognitive control. , Stimulants such as methylphenidate are increasingly used for cognitive enhancement but precise mechanisms are unknown. We found that methylphenidate boosts willingness to expend cognitive effort by altering the benefit-to-cost ratio of cognitive work. Willingness to expend effort was greater for participants with higher striatal dopamine synthesis capacity, whereas methylphenidate and sulpiride, a selective D2 receptor antagonist, increased cognitive motivation more for participants with lower synthesis capacity. A sequential sampling model informed by momentary gaze revealed that decisions to expend effort are related to amplification of benefit-versus-cost information attended early in the decision process, whereas the effect of benefits is strengthened with higher synthesis capacity and by methylphenidate. These findings demonstrate that methylphenidate boosts the perceived benefits versus costs of cognitive effort by modulating striatal dopamine signaling.}, language = {en}, number = {6484}, urldate = {2023-06-02}, journal = {Science}, author = {Westbrook, A. and Van Den Bosch, R. and Määttä, J. I. and Hofmans, L. and Papadopetraki, D. and Cools, R. and Frank, M. J.}, month = mar, year = {2020}, keywords = {Decision making, Neuroscience, unread}, pages = {1362--1366}, }
@article{peek_brain_2020, title = {Brain {GABA} and glutamate levels across pain conditions: {A} systematic literature review and meta-analysis of {1H}-{MRS} studies using the {MRS}-{Q} quality assessment tool}, volume = {210}, issn = {1053-8119}, shorttitle = {Brain {GABA} and glutamate levels across pain conditions}, url = {https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053811920300197}, doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116532}, abstract = {Background A proposed mechanism of chronic pain is dysregulation between the main inhibitory (GABA) and excitatory (glutamate) neurometabolites of the central nervous system. The level of these neurometabolites appears to differ in individual studies of people with pain compared to pain-free controls across different pain conditions. However, this has yet to be systematically investigated. Aims To establish whether GABA, glutamate, glutamine and Glx levels differ across pain conditions when compared to pain-free controls. Methods Five databases were searched. Studies were included if they investigated: 1) A pain condition compared to control. 2) Reported GABA, glutamate, glutamine or glutamate/glutamine level. 3) Used 1H-Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy (Prospero Project ID CRD42018092170). Data extracted included neurometabolite level, pain diagnosis, and spectroscopy parameters. Meta-analyses were conducted to establish the difference in neurometabolite level between participants with pain and pain-free controls for different pain conditions. The MRS-Q was developed from existing clinical consensus to allow for the assessment of quality in the included studies. Results Thirty-five studies were included investigating combinations of migraine (n = 11), musculoskeletal pain (n = 8), chronic pain syndromes (n = 9) and miscellaneous pain (n = 10). Higher GABA levels were found in participants with migraine compared to controls (Hedge's G 0.499, 95\%CI: 0.2 to 0.798). In contrast, GABA levels in musculoskeletal pain conditions (Hedge's G −0.189, 95\%CI: 0.530 to 0.153) and chronic pain syndromes (Hedge's G 0.077, 95\%CI: 1.612 to 1.459) did not differ from controls. Results for other brain neurometabolites revealed significantly higher levels for glutamate in participants with migraine and Glx in chronic pain syndromes compared to controls. Conclusion These results support the theory that underlying neurometabolite levels may be unique in different pain conditions and therefore representative of biomarkers for specific pain conditions.}, language = {en}, urldate = {2023-06-22}, journal = {NeuroImage}, author = {Peek, Aimie Laura and Rebbeck, Trudy and Puts, Nicolaas AJ. and Watson, Julia and Aguila, Maria-Eliza R. and Leaver, Andrew M.}, month = apr, year = {2020}, keywords = {unread}, pages = {116532}, }
@article{harris_potential_2019, title = {The potential power of experience in communications of expert consensus levels}, volume = {22}, issn = {14664461}, url = {https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2018.1440416}, doi = {10.1080/13669877.2018.1440416}, abstract = {Understanding the scientific consensus on anthropogenic climate change has been dubbed a ‘gateway belief’ to engaging people in sustainable behaviour. We consider the question of how the impact of a consensus communication can be maximised. Firstly, the credibility of the communicator should be maximised. One way of achieving this is to present the opinions of a sample of scientists directly to individuals. The decision-making literature suggests that such a technique will confer an additional advantage over standard descriptions of consensus (e.g. ‘97\% of scientists agree’). In decision-making research, low probabilities tend to be overweighted when probabilities are described, but underweighted when probability information is experienced. Consequently, statements of high consensus may lead to an overweighting of the dissensus, a phenomenon that may be reversed were the consensus to be ‘experienced.’ We obtain some positive support for our proposal that consensus is best ‘experienced’ in one of two experiments. We suggest that the lack of stronger positive support could relate to ceiling effects for the topics studied and propose that investigation of effective methods for ‘experiencing’ the consensus is a fruitful area for future research.}, number = {5}, journal = {Journal of Risk Research}, author = {Harris, Adam J.L. and Sildmäe, Oliver and Speekenbrink, Maarten and Hahn, Ulrike}, year = {2019}, note = {Publisher: Routledge}, keywords = {Science communication, unread}, pages = {593--609}, }
@article{oberauer_addressing_2019, title = {Addressing the theory crisis in psychology}, volume = {26}, issn = {1069-9384, 1531-5320}, url = {http://link.springer.com/10.3758/s13423-019-01645-2}, doi = {10.3758/s13423-019-01645-2}, abstract = {A worrying number of psychological findings are not replicable. Diagnoses of the causes of this “replication crisis,” and recommendations to address it, have nearly exclusively focused on methods of data collection, analysis, and reporting. We argue that a further cause of poor replicability is the often weak logical link between theories and their empirical tests. We propose a distinction between discovery-oriented and theory-testing research. In discovery-oriented research, theories do not strongly imply hypotheses by which they can be tested, but rather define a search space for the discovery of effects that would support them. Failures to find these effects do not question the theory. This endeavor necessarily engenders a high risk of Type I errors—that is, publication of findings that will not replicate. Theory-testing research, by contrast, relies on theories that strongly imply hypotheses, such that disconfirmation of the hypothesis provides evidence against the theory. Theory-testing research engenders a smaller risk of Type I errors. A strong link between theories and hypotheses is best achieved by formalizing theories as computational models. We critically revisit recommendations for addressing the “replication crisis,” including the proposal to distinguish exploratory from confirmatory research, and the preregistration of hypotheses and analysis plans.}, language = {en}, number = {5}, urldate = {2023-06-02}, journal = {Psychonomic Bulletin \& Review}, author = {Oberauer, Klaus and Lewandowsky, Stephan}, month = oct, year = {2019}, keywords = {Meta-research, Methodology, Philosophy, Replication, unread}, pages = {1596--1618}, }
@article{niewiadomska_population-level_2019, title = {Population-level mathematical modeling of antimicrobial resistance: a systematic review}, volume = {17}, issn = {1741-7015}, shorttitle = {Population-level mathematical modeling of antimicrobial resistance}, url = {https://bmcmedicine.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/s12916-019-1314-9}, doi = {10.1186/s12916-019-1314-9}, abstract = {Background: Mathematical transmission models are increasingly used to guide public health interventions for infectious diseases, particularly in the context of emerging pathogens; however, the contribution of modeling to the growing issue of antimicrobial resistance (AMR) remains unclear. Here, we systematically evaluate publications on population-level transmission models of AMR over a recent period (2006–2016) to gauge the state of research and identify gaps warranting further work. Methods: We performed a systematic literature search of relevant databases to identify transmission studies of AMR in viral, bacterial, and parasitic disease systems. We analyzed the temporal, geographic, and subject matter trends, described the predominant medical and behavioral interventions studied, and identified central findings relating to key pathogens. Results: We identified 273 modeling studies; the majority of which ({\textgreater} 70\%) focused on 5 infectious diseases (human immunodeficiency virus (HIV), influenza virus, Plasmodium falciparum (malaria), Mycobacterium tuberculosis (TB), and methicillin-resistant Staphylococcus aureus (MRSA)). AMR studies of influenza and nosocomial pathogens were mainly set in industrialized nations, while HIV, TB, and malaria studies were heavily skewed towards developing countries. The majority of articles focused on AMR exclusively in humans (89\%), either in community (58\%) or healthcare (27\%) settings. Model systems were largely compartmental (76\%) and deterministic (66\%). Only 43\% of models were calibrated against epidemiological data, and few were validated against out-of-sample datasets (14\%). The interventions considered were primarily the impact of different drug regimens, hygiene and infection control measures, screening, and diagnostics, while few studies addressed de novo resistance, vaccination strategies, economic, or behavioral changes to reduce antibiotic use in humans and animals. Conclusions: The AMR modeling literature concentrates on disease systems where resistance has been longestablished, while few studies pro-actively address recent rise in resistance in new pathogens or explore upstream strategies to reduce overall antibiotic consumption. Notable gaps include research on emerging resistance in Enterobacteriaceae and Neisseria gonorrhoeae; AMR transmission at the animal-human interface, particularly in agricultural and veterinary settings; transmission between hospitals and the community; the role of environmental factors in AMR transmission; and the potential of vaccines to combat AMR.}, language = {en}, number = {1}, urldate = {2022-09-15}, journal = {BMC Medicine}, author = {Niewiadomska, Anna Maria and Jayabalasingham, Bamini and Seidman, Jessica C. and Willem, Lander and Grenfell, Bryan and Spiro, David and Viboud, Cecile}, month = dec, year = {2019}, keywords = {unread, ⛔ No INSPIRE recid found}, pages = {81}, file = {Niewiadomska et al. - 2019 - Population-level mathematical modeling of antimicr.pdf:C\:\\Users\\matth\\Zotero\\storage\\X52HXQVR\\Niewiadomska et al. - 2019 - Population-level mathematical modeling of antimicr.pdf:application/pdf}, }
@article{poldrack_importance_2019, title = {The {Importance} of {Standards} for {Sharing} of {Computational} {Models} and {Data}}, volume = {2}, issn = {2522-0861, 2522-087X}, url = {http://link.springer.com/10.1007/s42113-019-00062-x}, doi = {10.1007/s42113-019-00062-x}, abstract = {The target article by Lee et al. (in review) highlights the ways in which ongoing concerns about research reproducibility extend to model-based approaches in cognitive science. Whereas Lee et al. focus primarily on the importance of research practices to improve model robustness, we propose that the transparent sharing of model specifications, including their inputs and outputs, is also essential to improving the reproducibility of model-based analyses. We outline an ongoing effort (within the context of the Brain Imaging Data Structure community) to develop standards for the sharing of the structure of computational models and their outputs.}, language = {en}, number = {3-4}, urldate = {2023-06-02}, journal = {Computational Brain \& Behavior}, author = {Poldrack, Russell A and Feingold, Franklin and Frank, Michael J and Gleeson, Padraig and De Hollander, Gilles and Huys, Quentin J. M. and Love, Bradley C. and Markiewicz, Christopher J. and Moran, Rosalyn and Ritter, Petra and Rogers, Timothy T. and Turner, Brandon M. and Yarkoni, Tal and Zhan, Ming and Cohen, Jonathan D.}, month = dec, year = {2019}, keywords = {Methodology, Neuroimaging, Neuroscience, Replication, unread}, pages = {229--232}, }
@book{bentley_1950s_2018, title = {The 1950s: {A} {Decade} of {Modern} {British} {Fiction}}, isbn = {978-1-350-01152-6}, shorttitle = {The 1950s}, abstract = {How did social, cultural and political events in Britain during the 1950s shape modern British fiction? As Britain emerged from the shadow of war into the new decade of the 1950s, the seeds of profound social change were being sown. Exploring the full range of fiction in the 1950s, this volume surveys the ways in which these changes were reflected in British culture. Chapters cover the rise of the 'Angry Young Men', an emerging youth culture and vivid new voices from immigrant and feminist writers. A major critical re-evaluation of the decade, the book covers such writers as Margery Allingham, Kingsley Amis, E. R. Braithwaite, Rodney Garland, Martyn Goff, Attia Hosain, George Lamming, Marghanita Laski, Doris Lessing, Colin MacInnes, Naomi Mitchison, V. S. Naipaul, Barbara Pym, Mary Renault, Sam Selvon, Alan Sillitoe, John Sommerfield, Muriel Spark, J. R. R. Tolkien, Angus Wilson and John Wyndham.}, language = {en}, publisher = {Bloomsbury Publishing}, author = {Bentley, Nick and Ferrebe, Alice and Hubble, Nick}, month = sep, year = {2018}, note = {ZSCC: NoCitationData[s0] Google-Books-ID: p4hoDwAAQBAJ}, keywords = {unread} }
@article{west_survival_2018, title = {Survival behavior in the cyclic {Lotka}-{Volterra} model with a randomly switching reaction rate}, volume = {97}, issn = {2470-0045, 2470-0053}, url = {https://link.aps.org/doi/10.1103/PhysRevE.97.022406}, doi = {10.1103/PhysRevE.97.022406}, language = {en}, number = {2}, urldate = {2022-02-11}, journal = {Physical Review E}, author = {West, Robert and Mobilia, Mauro and Rucklidge, Alastair M.}, month = feb, year = {2018}, keywords = {unread}, pages = {022406}, }
@article{applegate_cultural_2018, title = {Cultural {History}: {Where} {It} {Has} {Been} and {Where} {It} {Is} {Going}}, volume = {51}, issn = {0008-9389, 1569-1616}, shorttitle = {Cultural {History}}, url = {http://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/central-european-history/article/cultural-history-where-it-has-been-and-where-it-is-going/2D6A979327BCA44B88441B315C335042}, doi = {10.1017/S0008938918000122}, abstract = {The very meaning of “culture” has gone through so many transformations over the last sixty years that it is necessary to take stock of developments in this field of cultural history before suggesting—with an eye to the promises and perils of earlier practices—what new possibilities might exist for the future of the field. The post-1945 period witnessed a powerful impulse to understand culture as something more pervasive than just literature and the arts—and as something more socially and politically reverberant than the shibboleth of “art for art's sake.” In 1957, at the very beginning of the modern practice of cultural history, Richard Hoggart's The Uses of Literacy found the high and low hierarchies embedded in it. It focused on working-class culture (e.g., glossy magazines, films, “penny dreadfuls”), and on how reading was changing under the impact of mass media. By 1976, Raymond Williams needed to draw attention to the complexity of the word culture, so extended had its purview become over the previous two decades. Linda Nochlin asked why they were no great women artists, and T. J. Clark, using a Marxist framework, sought to understand aesthetic modernism by interrogating the historic circumstances that had led to the breakdown of the academic system. The New Cultural History, edited by Lynn Hunt, came out in 1989. Its “models” for cultural history were the work of Michel Foucault, Clifford Geertz, Natalie Zemon Davis, E. P. Thompson, Hayden White, and Dominick LaCapra, and its “new approaches” came from Mary Ryan, Roger Chartier, Thomas Laqueur, and Randolph Starn. These scholars were legislators of discourse and narrative, of popular and working-class culture, of gender, epistemes, and thick description. With many other tendencies, often defined by their focus on theoretical explication and elaboration, these approaches had the effect of deterring scholars from reengaging with the traditional interests—even the raison d'etre—of cultural history, namely, art, architecture, theater, dance, music, and literature. This turning-away also affected the very composition of humanities and interpretive social science departments, which added many new subjects of study but, inevitably perhaps, let others wither away.}, language = {en}, number = {1}, urldate = {2018-05-13}, journal = {Central European History}, author = {Applegate, Celia and Potter, Pamela}, month = mar, year = {2018}, note = {ZSCC: 0000002}, keywords = {Culture, chapter one, unread}, pages = {75--82} }
@article{harris_edited_2017, title = {Edited {1H} magnetic resonance spectroscopy in vivo: {Methods} and metabolites}, volume = {77}, issn = {1522-2594}, shorttitle = {Edited {1H} magnetic resonance spectroscopy in vivo}, url = {http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1002/mrm.26619}, doi = {10.1002/mrm.26619}, abstract = {The Proton magnetic resonance (1H-MRS) spectrum contains information about the concentration of tissue metabolites within a predefined region of interest (a voxel). The conventional spectrum in some cases obscures information about less abundant metabolites due to limited separation and complex splitting of the metabolite peaks. One method to detect these metabolites is to reduce the complexity of the spectrum using editing. This review provides an overview of the one-dimensional editing methods available to interrogate these obscured metabolite peaks. These methods include sequence optimizations, echo-time averaging, J-difference editing methods (single BASING, dual BASING, and MEGA-PRESS), constant-time PRESS, and multiple quantum filtering. It then provides an overview of the brain metabolites whose detection can benefit from one or more of these editing approaches, including ascorbic acid, γ-aminobutyric acid, lactate, aspartate, N-acetyl aspartyl glutamate, 2-hydroxyglutarate, glutathione, glutamate, glycine, and serine. Magn Reson Med 77:1377–1389, 2017. © 2017 International Society for Magnetic Resonance in Medicine.}, language = {en}, number = {4}, urldate = {2023-02-09}, journal = {Magnetic Resonance in Medicine}, author = {Harris, Ashley D. and Saleh, Muhammad G. and Edden, Richard A.E.}, year = {2017}, note = {\_eprint: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/10.1002/mrm.26619}, keywords = {unread}, pages = {1377--1389}, }
@article{poldrack_scanning_2017, title = {Scanning the horizon: towards transparent and reproducible neuroimaging research}, volume = {18}, issn = {1471-003X, 1471-0048}, shorttitle = {Scanning the horizon}, url = {https://www.nature.com/articles/nrn.2016.167}, doi = {10.1038/nrn.2016.167}, abstract = {Functional neuroimaging techniques have transformed our ability to probe the neurobiological basis of behaviour and are increasingly being applied by the wider neuroscience community. However, concerns have recently been raised that the conclusions that are drawn from some human neuroimaging studies are either spurious or not generalizable. Problems such as low statistical power, flexibility in data analysis, software errors and a lack of direct replication apply to many fields, but perhaps particularly to functional MRI. Here, we discuss these problems, outline current and suggested best practices, and describe how we think the field should evolve to produce the most meaningful and reliable answers to neuroscientific questions.}, language = {en}, number = {2}, urldate = {2023-06-02}, journal = {Nature Reviews Neuroscience}, author = {Poldrack, Russell A. and Baker, Chris I. and Durnez, Joke and Gorgolewski, Krzysztof J. and Matthews, Paul M. and Munafò, Marcus R. and Nichols, Thomas E. and Poline, Jean-Baptiste and Vul, Edward and Yarkoni, Tal}, month = feb, year = {2017}, keywords = {Meta-research, Methodology, Replication, unread}, pages = {115--126}, }
@article{sutcliffe-braithwaite_discourses_2017, title = {Discourses of ‘class’ in {Britain} in ‘{New} {Times}’}, volume = {31}, issn = {1361-9462}, url = {https://doi.org/10.1080/13619462.2017.1306199}, doi = {10.1080/13619462.2017.1306199}, abstract = {In the 1980s, the New Times writers suggested that economic and cultural changes were ushering in an era where diverse identities were replacing ‘class’. This was a vision which the Labour Party feared, but which Margaret Thatcher embraced. In her rhetoric, she celebrated what she saw as a decline in the significance of class identities and distinctions. This article examines the place and meanings of ‘class’ in subjective accounts of ‘class’ in the period, from ‘ordinary’ individuals. It argues that many people, when asked about ‘class’ in interviews or by Mass Observation, foregrounded ‘ordinariness’ as central to their identity. Many took ‘seeing people as members of a class’ to mean snobbishness or deference. ‘Class’ was frequently taken to mean pretentiousness and/or inferiority—and this was a highly negative meaning. Many asserted that they themselves had, or at least aspired to, a ‘classless’, socially egalitarian outlook, and were neither snobbish nor deferential. However, most people also identified and critiqued large, structural inequalities in British society. The place of ‘class’ in British society and culture in this period remained complex and often contradictory.}, number = {2}, urldate = {2018-05-17}, journal = {Contemporary British History}, author = {Sutcliffe-Braithwaite, Florence}, month = apr, year = {2017}, note = {ZSCC: 0000012 Citation Key Alias: sutcliffe-braithwaiteDiscoursesClassBritain2017a}, keywords = {Class, Labour Party, Thatcher, deference, more than 5 citations, subjectivities, table of contents, unread}, pages = {294--317} }
@article{van_leeuwen__2016, title = {" {En} {Quatre} {Couleur}": {An} {Eighteenth}-{Century} {Gold} {Watch} {Chatelaine} in the {Rijksmuseum}}, volume = {64}, shorttitle = {" {En} {Quatre} {Couleur}"}, number = {4}, journal = {The Rijksmuseum Bulletin}, author = {Van Leeuwen, Suzanne}, year = {2016}, keywords = {Versailles plate project, outside France, unread}, pages = {328--346}, }
@article{jiang_impacts_2015, title = {Impacts of {Emerging} {Contaminants} on {Surrounding} {Aquatic} {Environment} from a {Youth} {Festival}}, volume = {49}, issn = {0013-936X, 1520-5851}, url = {https://pubs.acs.org/doi/10.1021/es503944e}, doi = {10.1021/es503944e}, abstract = {The youth festival as we refer to Spring Scream, a large-scale pop music festival, is notorious for the problems of drug abuse and addiction. The origin, temporal magnitudes, potential risks and mass inputs of emerging contaminants (ECs) were investigated. Thirty targeted ECs were analyzed by solid-phase extraction and liquid chromatography coupled to tandem mass spectrometry (SPE-LC-MS/MS). Sampling strategy was designed to characterize EC behavior in different stages (before and after the youth festival), based on multivariate data analysis to explore the contributions of contaminants from normal condition to the youth festival. Wastewater influents and effluents were collected during the youth festival (approximately 600 000 pop music fans and youth participated). Surrounding river waters are also sampled to illustrate the touristic impacts during peak season and off-season. Seasonal variations were observed, with the highest concentrations in April (Spring Scream) and the lowest in October (off-season). Acetaminophen, diclofenac, codeine, ampicillin, tetracycline, erythromycin-H2O, and gemfibrozil have significant pollution risk quotients (RQs {\textgreater} 1), indicating ecotoxicological concerns. Principal component analysis (PCA) and weekly patterns provide a perspective in assessing the touristic impacts and address the dramatic changes in visitor population and drug consumption. The highest mass loads discharged into the aquatic ecosystem corresponded to illicit drugs/controlled substances such as ketamine and MDMA, indicating the high consumption of ecstasy during Spring Scream.}, language = {en}, number = {2}, urldate = {2023-06-02}, journal = {Environmental Science \& Technology}, author = {Jiang, Jheng-Jie and Lee, Chon-Lin and Fang, Meng-Der and Tu, Bo-Wen and Liang, Yu-Jen}, month = jan, year = {2015}, keywords = {Health, unread}, pages = {792--799}, }
@article{hymers_neural_2015, title = {Neural mechanisms underlying song and speech perception can be differentiated using an illusory percept}, volume = {108}, issn = {10538119}, url = {https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1053811914010027}, doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.010}, abstract = {The issue of whether human perception of speech and song recruits integrated or dissociated neural systems is contentious. This issue is difficult to address directly since these stimulus classes differ in their physical attributes. We therefore used a compelling illusion (Deutsch et al. 2011) in which acoustically identical auditory stimuli are perceived as either speech or song. Deutsch's illusion was used in a functional MRI experiment to provide a direct, within-subject investigation of the brain regions involved in the perceptual transformation from speech into song, independent of the physical characteristics of the presented stimuli. An overall differential effect resulting from the perception of song compared with that of speech was revealed in right midposterior superior temporal sulcus/right middle temporal gyrus. A left frontotemporal network, previously implicated in higher-level cognitive analyses of music and speech, was found to co-vary with a behavioural measure of the subjective vividness of the illusion, and this effect was driven by the illusory transformation. These findings provide evidence that illusory song perception is instantiated by a network of brain regions that are predominantly shared with the speech perception network.}, language = {en}, urldate = {2023-06-02}, journal = {NeuroImage}, author = {Hymers, Mark and Prendergast, Garreth and Liu, Can and Schulze, Anja and Young, Michellie L. and Wastling, Stephen J. and Barker, Gareth J. and Millman, Rebecca E.}, month = mar, year = {2015}, keywords = {Hearing, Perception, unread}, pages = {225--233}, }
@article{hughes_evolutionary_2015, title = {Evolutionary consequences of drug resistance: shared principles across diverse targets and organisms}, volume = {16}, copyright = {2015 Nature Publishing Group, a division of Macmillan Publishers Limited. All Rights Reserved.}, issn = {1471-0064}, shorttitle = {Evolutionary consequences of drug resistance}, url = {https://www.nature.com/articles/nrg3922}, doi = {10.1038/nrg3922}, abstract = {Across the diverse biological systems discussed in this Review, the underlying principles concerning the mechanisms and dynamics of resistance development are similar.Drug resistance has emerged in all biological systems in which drugs are used as a standard therapeutic strategy to control infections or cancer. There is an urgent need not only to develop new drugs to support effective therapy but also to develop a better understanding of the underlying mechanisms and forces that drive resistance development.Large population sizes and/or high mutation rates ensure that the emergence of drug resistance is not limited by mutation supply in HIV, in many bacterial infections or in human cancers. Mutation supply may be a limiting factor for fungal and parasitic infections.Horizontal gene transfer (HGT) from a very broad gene pool substantially contributes to the emergence of drug resistance in bacteria but is absent as a source of genetic variation in the other systems discussed. We currently know very little about the dynamics and trajectories of HGT events and have a very poor ability to make predictions.The study and understanding of the dynamics of growth and competition within complex populations subjected to drug therapy are being advanced by the increasing application of next-generation sequencing technologies.In biological systems in which resistance emergence has long been acknowledged to be a problem (particularly HIV infection and human cancer), therapy with combinations of drugs is standard of care. The systematic use of drug combinations in the treatment of bacterial, fungal and parasitic infections might be the most effective short-term means to slow resistance emergence.}, language = {en}, number = {8}, urldate = {2022-07-02}, journal = {Nature Reviews Genetics}, author = {Hughes, Diarmaid and Andersson, Dan I.}, month = aug, year = {2015}, note = {Number: 8 Publisher: Nature Publishing Group}, keywords = {unread, ⛔ No INSPIRE recid found}, pages = {459--471}, file = {Full Text PDF:C\:\\Users\\matth\\Zotero\\storage\\UVSLQC6Z\\Hughes and Andersson - 2015 - Evolutionary consequences of drug resistance shar.pdf:application/pdf;Snapshot:C\:\\Users\\matth\\Zotero\\storage\\VA86K2W5\\nrg3922.html:text/html}, }
@article{oconnor_gender_2014, title = {Gender on the {Brain}: {A} {Case} {Study} of {Science} {Communication} in the {New} {Media} {Environment}}, volume = {9}, issn = {1932-6203}, shorttitle = {Gender on the {Brain}}, url = {https://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0110830}, doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0110830}, abstract = {Neuroscience research on sex difference is currently a controversial field, frequently accused of purveying a ‘neurosexism’ that functions to naturalise gender inequalities. However, there has been little empirical investigation of how information about neurobiological sex difference is interpreted within wider society. This paper presents a case study that tracks the journey of one high-profile study of neurobiological sex differences from its scientific publication through various layers of the public domain. A content analysis was performed to ascertain how the study was represented in five domains of communication: the original scientific article, a press release, the traditional news media, online reader comments and blog entries. Analysis suggested that scientific research on sex difference offers an opportunity to rehearse abiding cultural understandings of gender. In both scientific and popular contexts, traditional gender stereotypes were projected onto the novel scientific information, which was harnessed to demonstrate the factual truth and normative legitimacy of these beliefs. Though strains of misogyny were evident within the readers’ comments, most discussion of the study took pains to portray the sexes’ unique abilities as equal and ‘complementary’. However, this content often resembled a form of benevolent sexism, in which praise of women’s social-emotional skills compensated for their relegation from more esteemed trait-domains, such as rationality and productivity. The paper suggests that embedding these stereotype patterns in neuroscience may intensify their rhetorical potency by lending them the epistemic authority of science. It argues that the neuroscience of sex difference does not merely reflect, but can actively shape the gender norms of contemporary society.}, language = {en}, number = {10}, urldate = {2023-06-02}, journal = {PLoS ONE}, author = {O’Connor, Cliodhna and Joffe, Helene}, editor = {Boraud, Thomas}, month = oct, year = {2014}, keywords = {Equality, Science communication, unread}, pages = {e110830}, }
@article{orloff_pickles_2014, title = {Pickles and ice cream! {Food} cravings in pregnancy: hypotheses, preliminary evidence, and directions for future research}, volume = {5}, issn = {1664-1078}, shorttitle = {Pickles and ice cream! {Food} cravings in pregnancy}, url = {http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01076/abstract}, doi = {10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01076}, abstract = {Women in the United States experience an increase in food cravings at two specific times during their life, (1) perimenstrually and (2) prenatally. The prevalence of excess gestational weight gain (GWG) is a growing concern due to its association with adverse health outcomes in both mothers and children. To the extent that prenatal food cravings may be a determinant of energy intake in pregnancy, a better understanding of craving etiology could be crucial in addressing the issue of excessive GWG. This paper reviews the available literature to corroborate and/or dispute some of the most commonly accepted hypotheses regarding the causes of food cravings during pregnancy, including a role of (1) hormonal changes, (2) nutritional deficits, (3) pharmacologically active ingredients in the desired foods, and (4) cultural and psychosocial factors. An existing model of perimenstrual chocolate craving etiology serves to structure the discussion of these hypotheses.The main hypotheses discussed receive little support, with the notable exception of a postulated role of cultural and psychosocial factors.The presence of cravings during pregnancy is a common phenomenon across different cultures, but the types of foods desired and the adverse impact of cravings on health may be culture-specific. Various psychosocial factors appear to correlate with excess GWG, including the presence of restrained eating. Findings strongly suggest that more research be conducted in this area. We propose that future investigations fall into one of the four following categories: (1) validation of food craving and eatingrelated measures specifically in pregnant populations, (2) use of ecological momentary assessment to obtain real time data on cravings during pregnancy, (3) implementation of longitudinal studies to address causality between eating disorder symptoms, food cravings, and GWG, and (4) development of interventions to ensure proper prenatal nutrition and prevent excess GWG.}, language = {en}, urldate = {2023-06-02}, journal = {Frontiers in Psychology}, author = {Orloff, Natalia C. and Hormes, Julia M.}, month = sep, year = {2014}, keywords = {unread}, }
@article{clifford_is_2014, title = {Is {There} a {Cost} to {Convenience}? {An} {Experimental} {Comparison} of {Data} {Quality} in {Laboratory} and {Online} {Studies}}, volume = {1}, issn = {2052-2630, 2052-2649}, shorttitle = {Is {There} a {Cost} to {Convenience}?}, url = {https://www.cambridge.org/core/product/identifier/S2052263014000050/type/journal_article}, doi = {10.1017/xps.2014.5}, abstract = {Abstract Increasingly, experimental research is being conducted on the Internet in addition to the laboratory. Online experiments are more convenient for subjects and researchers, but we know little about how the choice of study location affects data quality. To investigate whether respondent behavior differs across study location, we randomly assign subjects to participate in a study in a laboratory or in an online setting. Contrary to our expectations, we find few differences between participants in terms of the level of attention and socially desirable responding. However, we find significant differences in two areas: the degree of self-reported distractions while completing the questionnaire and the tendency to consult outside sources for answers to political knowledge questions. We conclude that when the greater convenience (and higher response rates) of online experiments outweighs these disadvantages, Internet administration of randomized experiments represent an alternative to laboratory administration.}, language = {en}, number = {2}, urldate = {2023-06-02}, journal = {Journal of Experimental Political Science}, author = {Clifford, Scott and Jerit, Jennifer}, year = {2014}, keywords = {Methodology, unread}, pages = {120--131}, }
@article{simonsohn_p-curve_2014, title = {P-curve: {A} key to the file-drawer.}, volume = {143}, issn = {1939-2222, 0096-3445}, shorttitle = {P-curve}, url = {http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/a0033242}, doi = {10.1037/a0033242}, abstract = {Because scientists tend to report only studies (publication bias) or analyses (p-hacking) that “work,” readers must ask, “Are these effects true, or do they merely reflect selective reporting?” We introduce pcurve as a way to answer this question. P-curve is the distribution of statistically significant p values for a set of studies (ps .05). Because only true effects are expected to generate right-skewed p-curves—containing more low (.01s) than high (.04s) significant p values— only right-skewed p-curves are diagnostic of evidential value. By telling us whether we can rule out selective reporting as the sole explanation for a set of findings, p-curve offers a solution to the age-old inferential problems caused by file-drawers of failed studies and analyses.}, language = {en}, number = {2}, urldate = {2023-06-02}, journal = {Journal of Experimental Psychology: General}, author = {Simonsohn, Uri and Nelson, Leif D. and Simmons, Joseph P.}, year = {2014}, keywords = {Meta-research, Methodology, Replication, unread}, pages = {534--547}, }
@article{beaven_leisure_2013, title = {Leisure, citizenship and working-class men in {Britain}, 1850–1945}, url = {https://www.manchesterhive.com/view/9781847793607/9781847793607.xml}, language = {en\_US}, urldate = {2018-11-20}, author = {Beaven, Brad}, month = jul, year = {2013}, note = {ZSCC: 0000160}, keywords = {more than 5 citations, unread} }
@incollection{smith_gender_2013, address = {London}, title = {Gender {I}: {From} {Women}'s {History} to {Gender} {History}}, shorttitle = {Gender {I}}, url = {http://sk.sagepub.com/reference/hdbk_historicaltheory/n16.xml}, urldate = {2018-05-19}, booktitle = {The {SAGE} {Handbook} of {Historical} {Theory}}, publisher = {SAGE Publications Ltd}, author = {Smith, Bonnie G.}, year = {2013}, doi = {10.4135/9781446247563}, note = {ZSCC: 0000002 Citation Key Alias: smithGenderWomenHistory2013a}, keywords = {fantasies, gender, transsexualism, unread, women, women studies, working women, world history}, pages = {266--281} }
@article{brooke_slumming_2012, title = {'{Slumming}' in {Swinging} {London}?: {Class}, {Gender} and the {Post}-war {City} in {Nell} {Dunn}'s {Up} the {Junction} (1963).}, volume = {9}, issn = {14780038}, url = {https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=hia&AN=78362083&site=ehost-live}, abstract = {The present article uses Nell Dunn's Up the Junction (1963) to explore class, gender and the city in the 1960s. It focuses on three elements: the book's representation of post-war, urban working-class identity; the place of gender and sexuality within that representation; and, finally, Nell Dunn's own position as a middle-class observer. It argues for the continuing relevance and dynamism of class as a social referent in post-war, 'affluent' Britain. The article also explores the meaning of 'slumming' in the context of the mid-twentiethcentury city, against the background of 'affluence' and the emergence of the 'permissive society'. What becomes particularly apparent in both contexts is the importance of femininity and female sexuality in the representation of mid-twentieth-century London, whether in terms of the portrayal of working-class women or the position of the middle-class author. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]}, number = {3}, journal = {Cultural \& Social History}, author = {Brooke, Stephen}, month = sep, year = {2012}, note = {ZSCC: 0000015}, keywords = {more than 5 citations, unread}, pages = {429--449} }
@article{fox_efficacy_2012, series = {Novel {Pharmacotherapies} for {Depression}}, title = {Efficacy of {Transcranial} {Magnetic} {Stimulation} {Targets} for {Depression} {Is} {Related} to {Intrinsic} {Functional} {Connectivity} with the {Subgenual} {Cingulate}}, volume = {72}, issn = {0006-3223}, url = {https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0006322312004118}, doi = {10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.04.028}, abstract = {Background Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) is used clinically for the treatment of depression. However, the antidepressant mechanism remains unknown and its therapeutic efficacy remains limited. Recent data suggest that some left DLPFC targets are more effective than others; however, the reasons for this heterogeneity and how to capitalize on this information remain unclear. Methods Intrinsic (resting state) functional magnetic resonance imaging data from 98 normal subjects were used to compute functional connectivity with various left DLPFC TMS targets employed in the literature. Differences in functional connectivity related to differences in previously reported clinical efficacy were identified. This information was translated into a connectivity-based targeting strategy to identify optimized left DLPFC TMS coordinates. Results in normal subjects were tested for reproducibility in an independent cohort of 13 patients with depression. Results Differences in functional connectivity were related to previously reported differences in clinical efficacy across a distributed set of cortical and limbic regions. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex TMS sites with better clinical efficacy were more negatively correlated (anticorrelated) with the subgenual cingulate. Optimum connectivity-based stimulation coordinates were identified in Brodmann area 46. Results were reproducible in patients with depression. Conclusions Reported antidepressant efficacy of different left DLPFC TMS sites is related to the anticorrelation of each site with the subgenual cingulate, potentially lending insight into the antidepressant mechanism of TMS and suggesting a role for intrinsically anticorrelated networks in depression. These results can be translated into a connectivity-based targeting strategy for focal brain stimulation that might be used to optimize clinical response.}, language = {en}, number = {7}, urldate = {2022-10-12}, journal = {Biological Psychiatry}, author = {Fox, Michael D. and Buckner, Randy L. and White, Matthew P. and Greicius, Michael D. and Pascual-Leone, Alvaro}, month = oct, year = {2012}, keywords = {unread}, pages = {595--603}, }
@article{tauber_population_2012, title = {Population oscillations in spatial stochastic {Lotka}-{Volterra} models: {A} field-theoretic perturbational analysis}, volume = {45}, issn = {1751-8113, 1751-8121}, shorttitle = {Population oscillations in spatial stochastic {Lotka}-{Volterra} models}, url = {http://arxiv.org/abs/1206.2303}, doi = {10.1088/1751-8113/45/40/405002}, abstract = {Field theory tools are applied to analytically study fluctuation and correlation effects in spatially extended stochastic predator-prey systems. In the meanfield rate equation approximation, the classic Lotka–Volterra model is characterized by neutral cycles in phase space, describing undamped oscillations for both predator and prey populations. In contrast, Monte Carlo simulations for stochastic two-species predator-prey reaction systems on regular lattices display complex spatio-temporal structures associated with persistent erratic population oscillations. The Doi–Peliti path integral representation of the master equation for stochastic particle interaction models is utilized to arrive at a field theory action for spatial Lotka–Volterra models in the continuum limit. In the species coexistence phase, a perturbation expansion with respect to the nonlinear predation rate is employed to demonstrate that spatial degrees of freedom and stochastic noise induce instabilities toward structure formation, and to compute the fluctuation corrections for the oscillation frequency and diffusion coefficient. The drastic downward renormalization of the frequency and the enhanced diffusivity are in excellent qualitative agreement with Monte Carlo simulation data.}, language = {en}, number = {40}, urldate = {2022-09-16}, journal = {Journal of Physics A: Mathematical and Theoretical}, author = {Tauber, Uwe C.}, month = oct, year = {2012}, note = {arXiv:1206.2303 [cond-mat, q-bio]}, keywords = {Condensed Matter - Statistical Mechanics, Quantitative Biology - Populations and Evolution, unread}, pages = {405002}, }
@article{crum_mind_2011, title = {Mind over milkshakes: {Mindsets}, not just nutrients, determine ghrelin response.}, volume = {30}, issn = {1930-7810, 0278-6133}, shorttitle = {Mind over milkshakes}, url = {http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/a0023467}, doi = {10.1037/a0023467}, abstract = {Objective: To test whether physiological satiation as measured by the gut peptide ghrelin may vary depending on the mindset in which one approaches consumption of food. Methods: On 2 separate occasions, participants (n ϭ 46) consumed a 380-calorie milkshake under the pretense that it was either a 620-calorie “indulgent” shake or a 140-calorie “sensible” shake. Ghrelin was measured via intravenous blood samples at 3 time points: baseline (20 min), anticipatory (60 min), and postconsumption (90 min). During the first interval (between 20 and 60 min) participants were asked to view and rate the (misleading) label of the shake. During the second interval (between 60 and 90 min) participants were asked to drink and rate the milkshake. Results: The mindset of indulgence produced a dramatically steeper decline in ghrelin after consuming the shake, whereas the mindset of sensibility produced a relatively flat ghrelin response. Participants’ satiety was consistent with what they believed they were consuming rather than the actual nutritional value of what they consumed. Conclusions: The effect of food consumption on ghrelin may be psychologically mediated, and mindset meaningfully affects physiological responses to food.}, language = {en}, number = {4}, urldate = {2023-06-02}, journal = {Health Psychology}, author = {Crum, Alia J. and Corbin, William R. and Brownell, Kelly D. and Salovey, Peter}, year = {2011}, keywords = {Health, Perception, Taste, unread}, pages = {424--429}, }
@article{xavier_social_2011, title = {Social interaction in synthetic and natural microbial communities}, volume = {7}, issn = {1744-4292}, url = {https://www.embopress.org/doi/full/10.1038/msb.2011.16}, doi = {10.1038/msb.2011.16}, abstract = {Social interaction among cells is essential for multicellular complexity. But how do molecular networks within individual cells confer the ability to interact? And how do those same networks evolve from the evolutionary conflict between individual- and population-level interests? Recent studies have dissected social interaction at the molecular level by analyzing both synthetic and natural microbial populations. These studies shed new light on the role of population structure for the evolution of cooperative interactions and revealed novel molecular mechanisms that stabilize cooperation among cells. New understanding of populations is changing our view of microbial processes, such as pathogenesis and antibiotic resistance, and suggests new ways to fight infection by exploiting social interaction. The study of social interaction is also challenging established paradigms in cancer evolution and immune system dynamics. Finding similar patterns in such diverse systems suggests that the same ?social interaction motifs? may be general to many cell populations.}, number = {1}, urldate = {2022-09-27}, journal = {Molecular Systems Biology}, author = {Xavier, Joao B}, month = jan, year = {2011}, note = {Publisher: John Wiley \& Sons, Ltd}, keywords = {unread, ⛔ No INSPIRE recid found}, pages = {483}, file = {Full Text PDF:C\:\\Users\\matth\\Zotero\\storage\\Q3E8ZJ6R\\Xavier - 2011 - Social interaction in synthetic and natural microb.pdf:application/pdf}, }
@article{nowak_evolutionary_2010, title = {Evolutionary dynamics in structured populations}, volume = {365}, issn = {0962-8436}, url = {https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2842709/}, doi = {10.1098/rstb.2009.0215}, abstract = {Evolutionary dynamics shape the living world around us. At the centre of every evolutionary process is a population of reproducing individuals. The structure of that population affects evolutionary dynamics. The individuals can be molecules, cells, viruses, multicellular organisms or humans. Whenever the fitness of individuals depends on the relative abundance of phenotypes in the population, we are in the realm of evolutionary game theory. Evolutionary game theory is a general approach that can describe the competition of species in an ecosystem, the interaction between hosts and parasites, between viruses and cells, and also the spread of ideas and behaviours in the human population. In this perspective, we review the recent advances in evolutionary game dynamics with a particular emphasis on stochastic approaches in finite sized and structured populations. We give simple, fundamental laws that determine how natural selection chooses between competing strategies. We study the well-mixed population, evolutionary graph theory, games in phenotype space and evolutionary set theory. We apply these results to the evolution of cooperation. The mechanism that leads to the evolution of cooperation in these settings could be called ‘spatial selection’: cooperators prevail against defectors by clustering in physical or other spaces.}, number = {1537}, urldate = {2022-06-28}, journal = {Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences}, author = {Nowak, Martin A. and Tarnita, Corina E. and Antal, Tibor}, month = jan, year = {2010}, pmid = {20008382}, pmcid = {PMC2842709}, keywords = {unread, ⛔ No INSPIRE recid found}, pages = {19--30}, file = {PubMed Central Full Text PDF:C\:\\Users\\matth\\Zotero\\storage\\H4QG4XLB\\Nowak et al. - 2010 - Evolutionary dynamics in structured populations.pdf:application/pdf}, }
@article{krugman_theory_2010, title = {The theory of interstellar trade}, volume = {48}, issn = {00952583}, url = {https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1465-7295.2009.00225.x}, doi = {10.1111/j.1465-7295.2009.00225.x}, language = {en}, number = {4}, urldate = {2023-06-02}, journal = {Economic Inquiry}, author = {Krugman, Paul}, month = oct, year = {2010}, keywords = {Economics, unread}, pages = {1119--1123}, }
@article{thagard_why_2009, title = {Why {Cognitive} {Science} {Needs} {Philosophy} and {Vice} {Versa}}, volume = {1}, issn = {17568757, 17568765}, url = {https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01016.x}, doi = {10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01016.x}, abstract = {Contrary to common views that philosophy is extraneous to cognitive science, this paper argues that philosophy has a crucial role to play in cognitive science with respect to generality and normativity. General questions include the nature of theories and explanations, the role of computer simulation in cognitive theorizing, and the relations among the different fields of cognitive science. Normative questions include whether human thinking should be Bayesian, whether decision making should maximize expected utility, and how norms should be established. These kinds of general and normative questions make philosophical reflection an important part of progress in cognitive science. Philosophy operates best, however, not with a priori reasoning or conceptual analysis, but rather with empirically informed reflection on a wide range of findings in cognitive science.}, language = {en}, number = {2}, urldate = {2023-06-02}, journal = {Topics in Cognitive Science}, author = {Thagard, Paul}, month = apr, year = {2009}, keywords = {Meta-research, Methodology, Philosophy, unread}, pages = {237--254}, }
@article{hodgson_analysis_2009, title = {An {Analysis} of the {Concordance} {Among} 13 {U}.{S}. {Wine} {Competitions}}, volume = {4}, issn = {1931-4361, 1931-437X}, url = {https://www.cambridge.org/core/product/identifier/S1931436100000638/type/journal_article}, doi = {10.1017/S1931436100000638}, abstract = {Abstract An analysis of over 4000 wines entered in 13 U.S. wine competitions shows little concordance among the venues in awarding Gold medals. Of the 2,440 wines entered in more than three competitions, 47 percent received Gold medals, but 84 percent of these same wines also received no award in another competition. Thus, many wines that are viewed as extraordinarily good at some competitions are viewed as below average at others. An analysis of the number of Gold medals received in multiple competitions indicates that the probability of winning a Gold medal at one competition is stochastically independent of the probability of receiving a Gold at another competition, indicating that winning a Gold medal is greatly influenced by chance alone. (JEL Classification: D02, Q19)}, language = {en}, number = {1}, urldate = {2023-06-02}, journal = {Journal of Wine Economics}, author = {Hodgson, Robert T.}, year = {2009}, keywords = {Decision making, Perception, Taste, Wine, unread}, pages = {1--9}, }
@article{berr_zero-one_2009, title = {Zero-{One} {Survival} {Behavior} of {Cyclically} {Competing} {Species}}, volume = {102}, issn = {0031-9007, 1079-7114}, url = {https://link.aps.org/doi/10.1103/PhysRevLett.102.048102}, doi = {10.1103/PhysRevLett.102.048102}, language = {en}, number = {4}, urldate = {2022-02-11}, journal = {Physical Review Letters}, author = {Berr, Maximilian and Reichenbach, Tobias and Schottenloher, Martin and Frey, Erwin}, month = jan, year = {2009}, keywords = {unread}, pages = {048102}, }
@article{siegrist_expectations_2009, title = {Expectations influence sensory experience in a wine tasting}, volume = {52}, issn = {01956663}, url = {https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0195666309000166}, doi = {10.1016/j.appet.2009.02.002}, abstract = {Information about a product may shape consumers’ taste experience. In a wine tasting experiment, participants received (positive or negative) information about the wine prior to or after the tasting. When the information was given prior to the tasting, negative information about the wine resulted in lower ratings compared to the group that received positive information. No such effect was observed when participants received the information after the tasting but before they evaluated the wine. Results suggest that the information about the wine affected the experience itself and not only participants’ overall assessment of the wine after the tasting.}, language = {en}, number = {3}, urldate = {2023-06-02}, journal = {Appetite}, author = {Siegrist, Michael and Cousin, Marie-Eve}, month = jun, year = {2009}, keywords = {Cognitive biases, Decision making, Perception, Taste, Wine, unread}, pages = {762--765}, }
@article{kahneman_conditions_2009, title = {Conditions for intuitive expertise: {A} failure to disagree.}, volume = {64}, issn = {1935-990X, 0003-066X}, shorttitle = {Conditions for intuitive expertise}, url = {http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/a0016755}, doi = {10.1037/a0016755}, abstract = {This article reports on an effort to explore the differences between two approaches to intuition and expertise that are often viewed as conflicting: heuristics and biases (HB) and naturalistic decision making (NDM). Starting from the obvious fact that professional intuition is sometimes marvelous and sometimes flawed, the authors attempt to map the boundary conditions that separate true intuitive skill from overconfident and biased impressions. They conclude that evaluating the likely quality of an intuitive judgment requires an assessment of the predictability of the environment in which the judgment is made and of the individual’s opportunity to learn the regularities of that environment. Subjective experience is not a reliable indicator of judgment accuracy.}, language = {en}, number = {6}, urldate = {2023-06-02}, journal = {American Psychologist}, author = {Kahneman, Daniel and Klein, Gary}, year = {2009}, keywords = {Cognitive biases, Decision making, unread}, pages = {515--526}, }
@article{bernstein_how_2009, title = {How to {Tell} {If} a {Particular} {Memory} {Is} {True} or {False}}, volume = {4}, issn = {1745-6916, 1745-6924}, url = {http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01140.x}, doi = {10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01140.x}, abstract = {How can you tell if a particular memory belonging to you or someone else is true or false? Cognitive scientists use a variety of techniques to measure groups of memories, whereas police, lawyers, and other researchers use procedures to determine whether an individual can be believed or not. We discuss evidence from behavioral and neuroimaging studies and research on lying that have attempted to distinguish true from false memories.}, language = {en}, number = {4}, urldate = {2023-06-02}, journal = {Perspectives on Psychological Science}, author = {Bernstein, Daniel M. and Loftus, Elizabeth F.}, month = jul, year = {2009}, keywords = {Memory, unread}, pages = {370--374}, }
@article{Banville:2009ai, Author = {Banville, D.}, Date-Added = {2009-05-11 13:57:25 -0400}, Date-Modified = {2009-05-11 15:58:37 -0400}, Journal = {Curr.~Opin.~Drug Discov.~Devel.}, Keywords = {unread}, Pages = {376--387}, Title = {Mining Chemical and Biological Information from the Drug Literature}, Volume = {12}, Year = {2009}, Bdsk-File-1 = {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}}
@article{thornton_we_2009, title = {'{We} have no redress unless we strike': {Class}, {Gender} and {Activism} in the {Melbourne} {Tailoresses}' {Strike}, 1882-83.}, issn = {00236942}, url = {https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=hia&AN=43171170&site=ehost-live}, abstract = {By examining on the role played by rank-and-file activists and their leaders, this article seeks to balance the institutional focus of existing accounts of the 1883-3 Melbourne Tailoresses' Strike. Placing the strike in the broader context of the emergence of a woman-centred trade unionism, it argues that while the strike ultimately failed in its goal of improving the tailoresses' wages, it marked a major turning-point in public perceptions of female factory workers. Whereas the popular stereotype of the 'factory girl' was of a feckless adolescent who jeopardised her morals for a 'pernicious freedom', the striking tailoresses presented themselves as redoubtable activists, responsible for the support of young families, younger siblings and aging parents. In claiming the status of breadwinners in their own right, they challenged their exclusion from the 'brotherhood' of organised labour and won the support of the Victorian public. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]}, number = {96}, journal = {Labour History}, author = {Thornton, Danielle}, month = may, year = {2009}, note = {ZSCC: 0000005 Citation Key Alias: thorntonWeHaveNo2009a}, keywords = {more than 5 citations, unread}, pages = {19--38} }
@article{krams_experimental_2008, title = {Experimental evidence of reciprocal altruism in the pied flycatcher}, volume = {62}, issn = {1432-0762}, url = {https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-007-0484-1}, doi = {10.1007/s00265-007-0484-1}, abstract = {Although human behaviour abounds with reciprocal altruism, few examples exist documenting reciprocal altruism in animals. Recent non-experimental evidence suggests that reciprocal altruism may be more common in nature than previously documented. Here we present experimental evidence of mobbing behaviour, the joint assault on a predator in an attempt to drive it away, as reciprocal altruism in the breeding pied flycatcher (Ficedula hypoleuca). Given a choice, pied flycatchers assisted in mobbing initiated by co-operating neighbours and did not join in mobbing when initiated by conspecific neighbours which had defected from necessary assistance 1 h before. The results suggest the birds followed a ‘tit-for-tat’-like strategy and that mobbing behaviour of breeding birds may be explained in terms of reciprocal altruism.}, language = {en}, number = {4}, urldate = {2022-02-23}, journal = {Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology}, author = {Krams, Indrikis and Krama, Tatjana and Igaune, Kristine and Mänd, Raivo}, month = feb, year = {2008}, keywords = {unread}, pages = {599--605}, }
@article{lagnado_impact_2008, title = {The impact of discredited evidence}, volume = {15}, issn = {1069-9384, 1531-5320}, url = {http://link.springer.com/10.3758/PBR.15.6.1166}, doi = {10.3758/PBR.15.6.1166}, language = {en}, number = {6}, urldate = {2023-06-02}, journal = {Psychonomic Bulletin \& Review}, author = {Lagnado, David A. and Harvey, Nigel}, month = dec, year = {2008}, keywords = {Meta-research, Methodology, Replication, unread}, pages = {1166--1173}, }
@article{brey_technological_2008, title = {The {Technological} {Construction} of {Social} {Power}}, volume = {22}, issn = {0269-1728}, url = {http://www.informaworld.com.ezproxy.library.yorku.ca/10.1080/02691720701773551}, doi = {10.1080/02691720701773551}, number = {1}, urldate = {2009-07-01TZ}, journal = {Social Epistemology: A Journal of Knowledge, Culture and Policy}, author = {Brey, Philip}, year = {2008}, note = {00020}, keywords = {unread}, pages = {71} }
@misc{munger_french_2008, title = {French {Silver} in the {Seventeenth} and {Eighteenth} {Centuries} {\textbar} {Essay} {\textbar} {Heilbrunn} {Timeline} of {Art} {History} {\textbar} {The} {Metropolitan} {Museum} of {Art}}, url = {http://www.metmuseum.org/toah/hd/fsilv/hd_fsilv.htm}, abstract = {“Silver has always held an exalted position within the decorative arts.”}, urldate = {2017-06-06}, journal = {The Met’s Heilbrunn Timeline of Art History}, author = {Munger, Author: Jeffrey}, year = {2008}, keywords = {Silver, Versailles plate project, unread}, }
@article{savage_revisiting_2005, title = {Revisiting {Classic} {Qualitative} {Studies}}, volume = {30}, issn = {0172-6404}, url = {http://www.jstor.org/stable/20762015}, abstract = {This paper explores methodological issues regarding the revisiting of "classic" qualitative studies. Classic studies pose particular issues for secondary analysis. By virtue of being "classic", the findings and arguments of such studies define a subsequent "canon" of theoretical and methodological scholarship, and hence shape the thinking of subsequent researchers conducting secondary analysis. Secondary re-analysis therefore should be not only of the archived data itself, but of the published work itself, but this raises a host of complex methodological and ethical issues. Using my own reanalysis of Elizabeth BOTT's "Family and Social Network' archive, and John GOLDTHORPE and David LOCKWOOD's "Affluent Worker collection", I examine possible analytical strategies for re-analysis, including "debunking", the alternative of "sacralisation", and ways in which original data can be read "against the grain".}, number = {1 (111)}, urldate = {2019-11-10}, journal = {Historical Social Research / Historische Sozialforschung}, author = {Savage, Mike}, year = {2005}, note = {ZSCC: 0000104}, keywords = {1950s, more than 5 citations, surveys, unread}, pages = {118--139} }
@article{savage_working-class_2005, title = {Working-{Class} {Identities} in the 1960s: {Revisiting} the {Affluent} {Worker} {Study}}, volume = {39}, issn = {0038-0385}, shorttitle = {Working-{Class} {Identities} in the 1960s}, url = {https://doi.org/10.1177/0038038505058373}, doi = {10.1177/0038038505058373}, abstract = {This article reports a secondary analysis of the fieldnotes collected by Goldthorpe, Lockwood, Bechhofer and Platt as part of their studies of affluent workers in Luton in the early 1960s. I argue that the ideal type distinction between power, prestige and pecuniary images of society, elaborated by Lockwood, fails to recognize that money, power and status were often fused in the statements and attitudes of the workers they interviewed. I show that most respondents had a keen sense that dominant social classes existed. I go on to argue that the hesitations evident in the fieldnotes when respondents were asked about class were not due to defensiveness so much as fundamental differences in the way that the researchers and the workers thought about class. The central claim that respondents sought to elaborate was their ordinariness and individuality; findings which, when compared with recent research, suggest considerable continuities in popular identities.}, language = {en}, number = {5}, urldate = {2019-11-10}, journal = {Sociology}, author = {Savage, Mike}, month = dec, year = {2005}, note = {ZSCC: 0000179}, keywords = {1950s, more than 5 citations, surveys, unread}, pages = {929--946} }
@article{giles_narratives_2002, title = {Narratives of {Gender}, {Class}, and {Modernity} in {Women}'s {Memories} of {Mid}-{Twentieth} {Century} {Britain}.}, volume = {28}, issn = {00979740}, url = {https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=hia&AN=8509332&site=ehost-live}, abstract = {Discusses the life stories of Jean Slater (b. 1924) and Hannah Arkwright (b. 1920), two working-class women who grew up in Great Britain during the 1930's. The narrated memories of working-class women leads to a new understanding of the relationship between modernity and femininity. The memories of the two women presented in this article show that femininity is an artificial construct that shifts according to historical and social circumstances.}, number = {1}, journal = {Signs: Journal of Women in Culture \& Society}, author = {Giles, Judy}, month = sep, year = {2002}, note = {ZSCC: 0000043}, keywords = {more than 5 citations, unread}, pages = {21} }
@article{mackey_diversitydisturbance_2001, title = {The {Diversity}–{Disturbance} {Relationship}: {Is} {It} {Generally} {Strong} and {Peaked}?}, volume = {82}, issn = {1939-9170}, shorttitle = {The {Diversity}–{Disturbance} {Relationship}}, url = {https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1890/0012-9658%282001%29082%5B3479%3ATDDRII%5D2.0.CO%3B2}, doi = {10.2307/2680166}, abstract = {The contemporary literature accepts that disturbance strongly influences patterns of species diversity, and that the relationship is peaked, with a maximum at intermediate levels of disturbance. We tested this hypothesis using a compilation of published species diversity–disturbance relationships that were gleaned from a literature search of papers published from 1985 through 1996 and from references therein. We identified 116 species richness–, 53 diversity–, and 28 evenness–disturbance relationships in the literature, which we grouped according to shape of relationship (nonsignificant, peaked, negative monotonic, positive monotonic, or U-shaped). We tested the relationships between the strength and shapes of these relationships and attributes of the community, disturbance, and sampling and study design. Nonsignificant relationships were the most common, comprising 35\% of richness, 28\% of diversity, and 50\% of evenness studies. Peaked responses were reported in only 16\% of richness, 19\% of diversity, and 11\% of evenness cases. Explained variation in the three measures of diversity was variable among studies but averaged ∼50\%. It was higher when few samples and few disturbance levels were examined and when organisms within the samples were not exhaustively censused, suggesting that procedural artifact contributes to these relationships. Explained variation was also higher in studies in which disturbance was measured as a gradient of time passed since the last disturbance (meanr2 = 61\%), vs. studies of spatial variation in richness (meanr2 = 42\%). Peaked richness relationships had the greatest odds of being observed when sampled area and actual evapotranspiration were small, when disturbances were natural rather than anthropogenic in origin, and when few disturbance levels were examined. Thus, on average, diversity–disturbance relationships do not have consistently highr2 and are not as consistently peaked as the contemporary consensus would suggest.}, language = {en}, number = {12}, urldate = {2023-03-07}, journal = {Ecology}, author = {Mackey, Robin L. and Currie, David J.}, year = {2001}, note = {\_eprint: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/10.1890/0012-9658\%282001\%29082\%5B3479\%3ATDDRII\%5D2.0.CO\%3B2}, keywords = {unread, disturbance, diversity, diversity–disturbance relationship, intermediate-disturbance hypothesis, meta-analyses, sampling artifacts, sampling intensity, species diversity, species richness, temporal and spatial patterns, ⛔ No INSPIRE recid found}, pages = {3479--3492}, file = {Snapshot:C\:\\Users\\matth\\Zotero\\storage\\UXBX6PPS\\0012-9658(2001)082[3479TDDRII]2.0.html:text/html}, }
@article{hoenig_abuse_2001, title = {The {Abuse} of {Power}: {The} {Pervasive} {Fallacy} of {Power} {Calculations} for {Data} {Analysis}}, volume = {55}, issn = {0003-1305, 1537-2731}, shorttitle = {The {Abuse} of {Power}}, url = {http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1198/000313001300339897}, doi = {10.1198/000313001300339897}, language = {en}, number = {1}, urldate = {2023-06-02}, journal = {The American Statistician}, author = {Hoenig, John M and Heisey, Dennis M}, month = feb, year = {2001}, keywords = {Meta-research, Methodology, Statistics, unread}, pages = {19--24}, }
@article{beaven_blitz_1999, title = {The blitz, civilian morale and the city: mass-observation and working-class culture in {Britain}, 1940–41}, volume = {26}, issn = {09639268, 14698706}, url = {http://www.jstor.org/stable/44613021}, abstract = {[One of the most difficult concepts in both contemporary and academic accounts of the Second World War is that of civilian morale. This paper uses evidence from the Mass-Observation Archive to argue that understanding fluctuations in morale can only be understood through an exploration of working-class culture during the 1930s and 1940s. The paper examines difficulties of defining 'morale' and goes on to argue that the pattern of bombing in urban centres and the continuity of working-class institutions helped shape and maintain morale during the critical period of 1940\&\#x2013;41.]}, number = {1}, journal = {Urban History}, author = {Beaven, Brad and Griffiths, John}, year = {1999}, note = {ZSCC: 0000022}, keywords = {more than 5 citations, unread}, pages = {71--88} }
@book{benko_space_1997, title = {Space and social theory : interpreting modernity and postmodernity}, isbn = {978-0-631-19466-8}, shorttitle = {Space and social theory}, url = {https://trove.nla.gov.au/version/37062148}, abstract = {The last decade has been a decade of tremendous change across the board of the human and social sciences. Ancient certainties, trusted ideologies and tested methods all came under immense pressure once so-called 'postmodern' ideas and concepts gained wider currency particularly among those with an interest in social theory. No longer content with framing social reality according to the logic of one core metaphor, the human and social sciences both rediscovered the local particularity of truth where hitherto a general explanation was deemed sufficient. In short: the revitalizing and formative power of 'space' was acknowledged once again. More than ten years into the debate, the present collection of original essays seeks to assess both the impact and current state of the debate around postmodernism and the spatial social sciences. It aims not at solving contradictions and differences within the debate since such a claim would be both fruitless and immature; rather, it seeks to demonstrate the diversity of interpretations that has come about by the mutual discovery of postmodern discourses and human geography since the mid 1980s. Celebrations of postmodernity, the insistence of a continuation of modernity, interpretations of globally-emerging postmodern spaces, even the call for an analysis of hypermodernity thus coexist in the collection at hand. In-between the essays, a new discursive agenda for the spatial human sciences emerges: not to pave the way for a new orthodoxy but simply to allow for the recognition of new ideas taking root in today's academic environment. This book is at once critical, provocative and accessible. It will be widely welcomed by advanced students of spatial and social theory in geography and related disciplines. xvi, 400 p. : ill. ; 26 cm. Bürger, Christa, 1935-. Postmoderne. Postmoderne Sozialwissenschaften. Raum. Postmoderne. Moderne. Raumwahrnehmung. Soziologie. Postmodernismo -- Aspectos sociales. Civilización moderna. Géographie humaine -- Philosophie. Postmodernisme -- Aspect social. Civilisation moderne et contemporaine. Sociale geografie. Postmodernisme. Civilization, Modern. Human geography -- Philosophy. Postmodernism -- Social aspects.}, language = {English}, urldate = {2019-06-03}, publisher = {Oxford, England ; Cambridge, Mass. : Blackwell Publishers}, author = {Benko, Georges and Strohmayer, Ulf}, year = {1997}, note = {ZSCC: 0000008}, keywords = {more than 5 citations, unread} }
@article{brey_philosophy_1997, title = {Philosophy of {Technology} {Meets} {Social} {Constructivism}}, volume = {2}, url = {http://scholar.lib.vt.edu/ejournals/SPT/v2_n3n4html/brey.html}, number = {3-4}, urldate = {2009-10-17TZ}, journal = {Society for Philosophy and Technology}, author = {Brey, Philip}, year = {1997}, keywords = {constructivism, philosophy of technology, technology, unread} }
@article{bordereau_suicidal_1997, title = {Suicidal defensive behaviour by frontal gland dehiscence in {Globitermes} sulphureus {Haviland} soldiers ({Isoptera})}, volume = {44}, issn = {1420-9098}, url = {https://doi.org/10.1007/s000400050049}, doi = {10.1007/s000400050049}, abstract = {Globitermes sulphureus is a well-known termite for the suicidal behaviour of the soldiers which liberate a sticky defensive secretion by rupturing their body. We have shown that this secretion is elaborated in a highly transformed frontal gland occupying a large part of the abdomen and the thorax, and not in the salivary glands as had been assumed until now. This special frontal gland without an outside opening is ruptured by violent contractions of the abdominal wall at the level of a weakness area of the sternal thoracic integument, just before the forecoxae. This soldier autothysis is efficient but represents only one of the components of the defensive strategy of the species which also uses a mechanical defense with mandibles and the intervention of an alarm pheromone recruiting congeners.}, language = {en}, number = {3}, urldate = {2022-02-23}, journal = {Insectes Sociaux}, author = {Bordereau, C. and Robert, A. and Van Tuyen, V. and Peppuy, A.}, month = aug, year = {1997}, keywords = {unread}, pages = {289--297}, }
@article{kuzon_seven_1996, title = {The {Seven} {Deadly} {Sins} of {Statistical} {Analysis}:}, volume = {37}, issn = {0148-7043}, shorttitle = {The {Seven} {Deadly} {Sins} of {Statistical} {Analysis}}, url = {http://journals.lww.com/00000637-199609000-00006}, doi = {10.1097/00000637-199609000-00006}, language = {en}, number = {3}, urldate = {2023-06-02}, journal = {Annals of Plastic Surgery}, author = {Kuzon, William M. and Urbanchek, Melanie G. and McCabe, Steven}, month = sep, year = {1996}, keywords = {Methodology, Statistics, unread}, pages = {265--272}, }
@article{birchall_co-partnership_1995, title = {Co-partnership housing and the garden city movement}, volume = {10}, url = {https://www.scopus.com/inward/record.uri?eid=2-s2.0-0029514880&doi=10.1080%2f02665439508725828&partnerID=40&md5=0672f1a637ce55acac20a24626d5d8a7}, doi = {10.1080/02665439508725828}, number = {4}, journal = {Planning Perspectives}, author = {Birchall, J.}, year = {1995}, note = {ZSCC: 0000020}, keywords = {more than 5 citations, unread}, pages = {329--358} }
@incollection{rendall_uneven_1991, title = {‘{Uneven} {Developments}’: {Women}’s {History}, {Feminist} {History} and {Gender} {History} in {Great} {Britain}}, booktitle = {Writing {Women}’s {History}}, publisher = {Springer}, author = {Rendall, Jane}, year = {1991}, note = {ZSCC: NoCitationData[s0]}, keywords = {unread}, pages = {45--57} }
@article{scott_deconstructing_1988, title = {Deconstructing {Equality}-versus-{Difference}: {Or}, the {Uses} of {Poststructuralist} {Theory} for {Feminism}}, volume = {14}, issn = {0046-3663}, shorttitle = {Deconstructing {Equality}-versus-{Difference}}, url = {http://www.jstor.org/stable/3177997}, doi = {10.2307/3177997}, number = {1}, urldate = {2018-05-13}, journal = {Feminist Studies}, author = {Scott, Joan W.}, year = {1988}, note = {ZSCC: 0001721}, keywords = {more than 5 citations, unread}, pages = {33--50} }
@book{armstrong_desire_1987, title = {Desire and {Domestic} {Fiction}: {A} {Political} {History} of the {Novel}}, isbn = {978-0-19-536474-3}, shorttitle = {Desire and {Domestic} {Fiction}}, abstract = {Desire and Domestic Fiction argues that far from being removed from historical events, novels by writers from Richardson to Woolf were themselves agents of the rise of the middle class. Drawing on texts that range from 18th-century female conduct books and contract theory to modern psychoanalytic case histories and theories of reading, Armstrong shows that the emergence of a particular form of female subjectivity capable of reigning over the household paved the way for the establishment of institutions which today are accepted centers of political power. Neither passive subjects nor embattled rebels, the middle-class women who were authors and subjects of the major tradition of British fiction were among the forgers of a new form of power that worked in, and through, their writing to replace prevailing notions of "identity" with a gender-determined subjectivity. Examining the works of such novelists as Samuel Richardson, Jane Austen, and the Brontës, she reveals the ways in which these authors rewrite the domestic practices and sexual relations of the past to create the historical context through which modern institutional power would seem not only natural but also humane, and therefore to be desired.}, language = {en}, publisher = {Oxford University Press}, author = {Armstrong, Nancy}, month = may, year = {1987}, note = {ZSCC: NoCitationData[s0] Google-Books-ID: YSrRCwAAQBAJ}, keywords = {unread} }
@article{bradbury_womens_1987, title = {Women's {History} and {Working}-{Class} {History}}, volume = {19}, issn = {07003862}, url = {http://www.jstor.org/stable/25142763}, abstract = {This paper seeks to examine the extent to which the writing of the history of both women and of the Canadian working class has converged over the last ten years, to suggest other ways in which integration of the two could be sought, and also to suggest some basic conflicts between the paradigms of each which point to areas where integration seems unlikely. It argues that if the goal of writing a history of the totality of the working class is a shared one, areas of intersection between the two fields must be consciously sought out. New ways of integrating the history of women and of the working class must be sought. For a start, this requires a reconceptualization of the way we define the working class and work, examination of the processes of class reproduction, and acknowledgement of the importance of examining how gender definitions are transmitted, shaped and reshaped. /// Cette article cherche \&\#xe0; examiner jusqu'\&\#xe0; quel point l'historiographie des femmes et de la classe ouvri\&\#xe8;re au Canada ont converg\&\#xe9; depuis dix ans, \&\#xe0; suggerer d'autres fa\&\#xe7;ons de les integrer tout en indiquant quelques conflits fondamentales dans les paradigmes de chaque champ d'histoire qui sugg\&\#xe8;rent des aires o\&\#xf9; l'int\&\#xe9;gration semble improbable. Il faut chercher consciemment les points de rencontre, d'intersection entre les deux champs et essayer de les integrer si on veut \&\#xe9;crire une histoire de toute la classe ouvri\&\#xe8;re. Au d\&\#xe9;part, il faut reconceptualiser notre fa\&\#xe7;on de definir la classe ouvri\&\#xe8;re ainsi que le travail, examiner comment les classe ouvri\&\#xe8;re ainsi que le travail, examiner comment les classes se reproduisent et reconna\&\#xee;tre l'importance de la transmission, de la formulation et la reformulation des definitions de genre.}, journal = {Labour / Le Travail}, author = {Bradbury, Bettina}, year = {1987}, note = {ZSCC: 0000061}, keywords = {more than 5 citations, unread}, pages = {23--43} }
@book{blumenfeld_jew_1986, title = {Jew {Boy}}, isbn = {978-0-85315-673-4}, abstract = {In 3 libraries. 348 p. ; 20cm.}, language = {English}, urldate = {2019-06-06}, publisher = {London : Lawrence \& Wishart}, author = {Blumenfeld, Simon}, year = {1986}, note = {ZSCC: NoCitationData[s0]}, keywords = {primary, unread} }
@article{scott_gender_1986, title = {Gender: {A} {Useful} {Category} of {Historical} {Analysis}}, volume = {91}, issn = {0002-8762}, shorttitle = {Gender}, url = {http://www.jstor.org/stable/1864376}, doi = {10.2307/1864376}, number = {5}, urldate = {2018-05-13}, journal = {The American Historical Review}, author = {Scott, Joan W.}, year = {1986}, note = {ZSCC: 0006582}, keywords = {more than 5 citations, unread}, pages = {1053--1075} }
@article{montagu_versailles._1986, title = {Versailles. {Paris} and {Versailles}}, volume = {128}, url = {http://www.jstor.org/stable/882448}, number = {996}, journal = {The Burlington Magazine}, author = {Montagu, Jennifer and Tadgell, Christopher}, month = mar, year = {1986}, keywords = {Versailles plate project, unread}, pages = {234--237}, }
@article{bauman_housing_1984, title = {Housing, {Architecture}, and {Social} {Change}: "{The} {British} and {American} {Experience}"}, volume = {10}, issn = {0096-1442}, url = {http://search.proquest.com.ezp.lib.unimelb.edu.au/docview/1297899042?accountid=12372}, number = {4}, journal = {Journal of Urban History}, author = {Bauman, John F}, month = aug, year = {1984}, note = {ZSCC: 0000001}, keywords = {History (General), unread}, pages = {465} }
@book{adam_im_1983, title = {I'm not complaining}, isbn = {978-0-86068-255-4}, url = {https://trove.nla.gov.au/version/45245467}, abstract = {In 9 libraries. xiv, 346 p. ; 20 cm. Novel -- English -- 20th century -- Texts. Fiction in English 1900-1945 Texts}, language = {English}, urldate = {2018-10-21}, publisher = {London : Virago}, author = {Adam, Ruth}, year = {1983}, note = {ZSCC: NoCitationData[s0]}, keywords = {primary, unread} }
@book{brierley_means-test_1983, title = {Means-test man}, isbn = {978-0-85124-377-1}, url = {https://trove.nla.gov.au/version/25902184}, abstract = {In 3 libraries. xvi, 281 p. ; 18 cm.}, language = {English}, urldate = {2019-05-26}, publisher = {Nottingham [Nottinghamshire] : Spokesman}, author = {Brierley, Walter}, year = {1983}, note = {ZSCC: NoCitationData[s0]}, keywords = {unread} }
@article{daniels_nest_1979, title = {Nest {Guard} {Replacement} in the {Antarctic} {Fish} {Harpagifer} bispinis: {Possible} {Altruistic} {Behavior}}, volume = {205}, shorttitle = {Nest {Guard} {Replacement} in the {Antarctic} {Fish} {Harpagifer} bispinis}, url = {https://www.science.org/doi/10.1126/science.205.4408.831}, doi = {10.1126/science.205.4408.831}, number = {4408}, urldate = {2022-06-29}, journal = {Science}, author = {Daniels, Robert A.}, month = aug, year = {1979}, note = {Publisher: American Association for the Advancement of Science}, keywords = {unread, ⛔ No INSPIRE recid found}, pages = {831--833}, file = {Full Text PDF:C\:\\Users\\matth\\Zotero\\storage\\C26JQ2FT\\Daniels - 1979 - Nest Guard Replacement in the Antarctic Fish Harpa.pdf:application/pdf}, }
@book{association_street_1977, title = {A street door of our own : a short history of life on an {LCC} estate}, isbn = {978-0-9506083-0-3}, shorttitle = {A street door of our own}, url = {https://trove.nla.gov.au/version/21439606}, abstract = {In 1 library. [1], 48p, [16]p of plates : ill., ports ; 22cm. Honor Oak Estate -- History. Honor Oak, Eng -- Social life and customs.}, language = {English}, urldate = {2019-01-06}, publisher = {[London] ([1-3 Forman House, Frensbury Rd, S.E.4]) : [Honor Oak Estate Neighbourhood Association]}, author = {Association, Honor Oak Estate Neighbourhood}, year = {1977}, note = {ZSCC: NoCitationData[s0]}, keywords = {unread} }
@article{grime_competitive_1973, title = {Competitive {Exclusion} in {Herbaceous} {Vegetation}}, volume = {242}, copyright = {1973 Nature Publishing Group}, issn = {1476-4687}, url = {https://www.nature.com/articles/242344a0}, doi = {10.1038/242344a0}, abstract = {IN maintaining or reconstructing types of herbaceous vegetation in which the density of flowering plants exceeds 20 species/m2—the so-called “species-rich” communities, success is often frustrated by competitive exclusion. Here I describe an attempt to identify criteria with which to assess or anticipate the effect of competitive exclusion both at individual sites and in different types of vegetation.}, language = {en}, number = {5396}, urldate = {2023-03-07}, journal = {Nature}, author = {Grime, J. P.}, month = mar, year = {1973}, note = {Number: 5396 Publisher: Nature Publishing Group}, keywords = {unread, Humanities and Social Sciences, multidisciplinary, Science, ⛔ No INSPIRE recid found}, pages = {344--347}, }
@book{abrams_must_1960, address = {Harmondsworth}, title = {Must {Labour} {Lose}?}, publisher = {Penguin Books}, author = {Abrams, Mark and Rose, Richard}, year = {1960}, note = {ZSCC: 0000302}, keywords = {1960s, more than 5 citations, unread} }
@article{bradbury_post-war_1956, title = {Post-{War} {Housing} in {Liverpool}}, volume = {27}, issn = {00410020, 1478341X}, url = {http://www.jstor.org/stable/40102221}, number = {3}, journal = {The Town Planning Review}, author = {Bradbury, Ronald}, year = {1956}, note = {ZSCC: 0000004}, keywords = {unread}, pages = {145--163} }
@article{arendt_ideology_1953, title = {Ideology and {Terror}: {A} {Novel} {Form} of {Government}}, volume = {15}, issn = {0034-6705, 1748-6858}, shorttitle = {Ideology and {Terror}}, url = {https://www.cambridge.org/core/product/identifier/S0034670500001510/type/journal_article}, doi = {10.1017/S0034670500001510}, abstract = {The following considerations have grown out of a study of the origins, the elements and the functioning of that novel form of government and domination which we have come to call totalitarian. Wherever it rose to power, it developed entirely new political institutions and destroyed all social, legal and political traditions of the country. No matter what the specifically national tradition or the particular spiritual source of its ideology, totalitarian government always transformed classes into masses, supplanted the party system, not by one-party dictatorships, but by a mass movement, shifted the center of power from the army to the police, and established a foreign policy openly directed toward world domination. Present totalitarian governments have developed from one-party systems; whenever these became truly totalitarian, they started to operate according to a system of values so radically different from all others, that none of our traditional legal, moral, or common sense utilitarian categories could any longer help us to come to terms with, or judge, or predict its course of action.}, language = {en}, number = {3}, urldate = {2023-06-02}, journal = {The Review of Politics}, author = {Arendt, Hannah}, month = jul, year = {1953}, keywords = {Philosophy, unread}, pages = {303--327}, }
@article{becker_housing_1951, title = {Housing in {England} and {Wales} during the {Business} {Depression} of the 1930's}, volume = {3}, issn = {0013-0117}, url = {http://www.jstor.org/stable/2599990}, doi = {10.2307/2599990}, number = {3}, urldate = {2019-08-23}, journal = {The Economic History Review}, author = {Becker, Arthur Peter}, year = {1951}, note = {ZSCC: 0000012}, keywords = {1930s, more than 5 citations, unread}, pages = {321--341} }
@article{bradbury_technique_1951, title = {The {Technique} of {Municipal} {Housing} in {England}: {With} {Particular} {Reference} to {Liverpool}}, volume = {22}, issn = {00410020, 1478341X}, url = {http://www.jstor.org/stable/40102159}, number = {1}, journal = {The Town Planning Review}, author = {Bradbury, Ronald}, year = {1951}, note = {ZSCC: 0000002}, keywords = {unread}, pages = {44--71} }
@article{beaufoy_well_1950, title = {Well {Hall} {Estate}, {Eltham}: {An} {Example} of {Good} {Housing} {Built} in 1915}, volume = {21}, issn = {00410020, 1478341X}, url = {http://www.jstor.org/stable/40102030}, number = {3}, journal = {The Town Planning Review}, author = {Beaufoy, S. L. G.}, year = {1950}, note = {ZSCC: NoCitationData[s0]}, keywords = {unread}, pages = {259--271} }
@book{abercrombie_town_1943, address = {London}, edition = {2nd ed}, series = {Home university library of modern knowledge}, title = {Town and country planning}, number = {163}, publisher = {Oxford University Press}, author = {Abercrombie, Patrick}, year = {1943}, note = {ZSCC: NoCitationData[s0]}, keywords = {unread} }
@article{buchanan_dog_1936, title = {The {Dog} in the {Manger}}, volume = {161}, issn = {0950-9852}, url = {http://search.proquest.com/britishperiodicals/docview/9625848/abstract/FE075C21C2D44199PQ/11}, abstract = {THE dangerous advance of socialism, which with a socialist as Prime Minister cannot cause surprise, has enveloped England and has produced an alarming discontent and restlessness among the working classes. Everything is done for their protection and comfort; Education! Insurance! The Dole! Hospitals!}, language = {English}, number = {4188}, urldate = {2019-05-23}, journal = {Saturday review of politics, literature, science and art; London}, author = {Buchanan, Meriel}, month = jan, year = {1936}, note = {ZSCC: NoCitationData[s0]}, keywords = {1930s, primary, unread}, pages = {38--39} }
@article{brickell_literary_1934, title = {The {Literary} {Landscape}}, volume = {237}, issn = {0029-2397}, url = {http://www.jstor.org/stable/25114436}, number = {4}, urldate = {2019-02-16}, journal = {The North American Review}, author = {Brickell, Herschel}, year = {1934}, note = {ZSCC: 0000004}, keywords = {unread}, pages = {376--384} }
@book{britton_hunger_1931, title = {Hunger and love}, url = {https://trove.nla.gov.au/version/45301888}, abstract = {In 4 libraries. xi, 705p. ; 21cm. Political fiction, English -- 20th century. Social conflict -- Great Britain -- Fiction. Working class -- Great Britain -- Fiction. Working class writings, English. Political fiction, English. Social conflict. Working class. Great Britain.}, language = {English}, urldate = {2019-05-26}, publisher = {London : Putnam}, author = {Britton, Lionel}, year = {1931}, note = {ZSCC: 0000003}, keywords = {SLV, unread} }
@article{adshead_standards_1925, title = {Standards of {Living} and {Architecture}}, volume = {4}, issn = {2043-1287}, url = {http://search.proquest.com/britishperiodicals/docview/6756426/abstract/7F6EEEE93B764F30PQ/40}, abstract = {OFTEN in the world of affairs the things which are most obvious are those which escape notice the easiest. How little we appreciate the effect of changed standards of living on the architecture of our time.}, language = {English}, number = {1}, urldate = {2019-05-23}, journal = {Architecture, a magazine of architecture and the applied arts and crafts; London}, author = {Adshead, S. D.}, month = may, year = {1925}, note = {ZSCC: 0000000}, keywords = {unread}, pages = {41--42} }
@misc{noauthor_psychological_nodate, title = {{SOME} {PSYCHOLOGICAL} {FACTORS} {IN} {HOME} {BUILDING} article for {Town} and {Country} {Planning}}, url = {http://www.massobservation.amdigital.co.uk/Documents/Details/FileReport-1622}, publisher = {Adam Matthew Digital}, keywords = {unread} }
@article{schmickl_strong_nodate, title = {Strong {Emergence} {Arising} from {Weak} {Emergence}}, abstract = {Predictions of emergent phenomena, appearing on the macroscopic layer of a complex system, can fail if they are made by a microscopic model. This study demonstrates and analyses this claim on a well-known complex system, Conway’s Game of Life. Straightforward macroscopic meanfield models are easily capable of predicting such emergent properties after they are fitted to simulation data in an after-the-fact way. Thus, these predictions are macro-to-macro only. However, a micro-to-macro model significantly fails to predict correctly, as does the obvious mesoscopic modeling approach. This suggests that some macroscopic system properties in a complex dynamic system should be interpreted as examples of phenomena (properties) arising from “strong emergence”, due to the lack of ability to build a consistent micro-tomacro model, that could explain these phenomena in a before-the-fact way. The root cause for this inability to predict this in a micro-to-macro way is identified as the pattern formation process, a phenomenon that is usually classified as being of “weak emergence”. Ultimately, this suggests that it may be in principle impossible to discriminate between such distinct categories of “weak” and “strong” emergence, as phenomena of both types can be part of the very same feedback loop that mainly governs the system’s dynamics.}, language = {en}, author = {Schmickl, Thomas}, keywords = {unread}, pages = {15}, }
@misc{noauthor_diversity_nodate, title = {Diversity in {Tropical} {Rain} {Forests} and {Coral} {Reefs} {\textbar} {Science}}, url = {https://www.science.org/doi/10.1126/science.199.4335.1302}, urldate = {2023-03-07}, keywords = {unread}, }
@misc{noauthor_potential_nodate, title = {Potential for adaptation overrides cost of resistance}, url = {https://www.futuremedicine.com/doi/epdf/10.2217/fmb.15.61}, language = {en}, urldate = {2022-09-27}, doi = {10.2217/fmb.15.61}, keywords = {unread}, }
@article{butler_self-organization_nodate, title = {Self-{Organization} {In} {Stellar} {Evolution}: {Size}-{Complexity} {Rule}}, abstract = {Complexity Theory is highly interdisciplinary, therefore any regularities must hold on all levels of organization, independent on the nature of the system. An open question in science is how complex systems self-organize to produce emergent structures and properties, a branch of non-equilibrium thermodynamics. It has long been known that there is a quantity-quality transition in natural systems. This is to say that the properties of a system depend on its size. More recently, this has been termed the size-complexity rule, which means that to increase their size, systems must increase their complexity, and that to increase their complexity they must grow in size. This rule goes under different names in different disciplines and systems of different nature, such as the areaspeciation rule, economies of scale, scaling relations (allometric) in biology and for cities, and many others. We apply the size-complexity rule to stars to compare them with other complex systems in order to find universal patterns of self-organization independent of the substrate. Here, as a measure of complexity of a star, we are using the degree of grouping of nucleons into atoms, which reduces nucleon entropy, increases the variety of elements, and changes the structure of the star. As seen in our previous work, complexity, using action efficiency, is in power law proportionality of all other characteristics of a complex system, including its size. Here we find that, as for the other systems studied, the complexity of stars is in a power law proportionality with their size - the bigger a system is, the higher its level of complexity is - despite differing explosion energies and initial metallicities from simulations and data, which confirms the size-complexity rule and our model.}, language = {en}, author = {Butler, Travis Herman and Georgiev, Georgi Yordanov}, keywords = {unread}, pages = {20}, }
@article{hernandez-navarro_exact_nodate, title = {Exact face landing probabilities for bouncing objects: edge probability in the coin toss and the three-sided die problem}, language = {en}, author = {Hernandez-Navarro, Lluıs and Pinero, Jordi}, keywords = {unread}, pages = {7}, }
@article{bles_data-driven_nodate, title = {A data-driven simulation of the trophallactic network and intranidal food flow dissemination in ants}, language = {en}, author = {Bles, Olivier and Deneubourg, Jean-Louis and Sueur, Cédric and Nicolis, Stamatios}, keywords = {unread}, pages = {33}, }